2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.03.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sex differences in unfamiliar face identification: Evidence from matching tasks

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
64
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 69 publications
(71 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
7
64
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although previous studies investigating sex differences in face processing have reported mixed results, they largely point to a female superiority (Megreya et al, 2011;Sommer et al, 2013). The observed sex differences for the Mooney test thus could reflect other, non-face-perception processes that might be at play.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 60%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Although previous studies investigating sex differences in face processing have reported mixed results, they largely point to a female superiority (Megreya et al, 2011;Sommer et al, 2013). The observed sex differences for the Mooney test thus could reflect other, non-face-perception processes that might be at play.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 60%
“…It is curious, for example, that males outperform females on the Mooney test (Foreman, 1991;Verhallen et al, 2014), whereas, if a sex difference is observed in other tests of face processing, it is in favor of females (Megreya, Bindemann, & Havard, 2011). Moreover, we have found (Verhallen et al, in preparation) that performance on a three-alternative forced-choice (3AFC) version of the original Mooney test does not correlate very strongly (Spearman's ρ = .21) with performance on a test of face discrimination (the Glasgow Face Matching Test; Burton, White, & McNeill, 2010), and correlates only modestly (Spearman's ρ = .31) with performance on a test of face recognition (the Cambridge Face Memory Test; Duchaine & Nakayama, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We should first note that our task was designed to measure people's tendency to rely on summary representations in face processing, rather than face recognition per se, and, as such, was different from the tasks used in previous work on the own-gender bias, such as tasks for face recognition memory (e.g., Cross et al, 1971) or simultaneous face-matching tasks (e.g., Megreya, Bindemann, & Havard, 2011). Apart from methodological factors, what could explain the greater averaging for owngender faces?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some studies showed that females perform better than males at face processing tasks (e.g., Bowles et al, 2009;Heisz, Pottruff, & Shore, 2013;McBain, Norton, & Chen, 2009;Megreya, Bindemann, & Havard, 2011;Rehnman & Herlitz 2007;Sommer, Hildebrandt, Kunina-Habenicht, Schacht, & Wilhelm, 2013), although this female advantage was not found in all studies, in particular in the original report of the BFRT (Benton & Van Allen, 1968). This advantage is also observed sometimes only in specific conditions (see Weirich, Hoffmann, Meißner, Heinz, & Bengner, 2011), could be specific for female faces (the so-called "female own-sex bias"; see e.g., Lewin & Herlitz, 2002;Lovén, Herlitz, & Rehnman, 2011;McKelvie, Standing, St Jean, & Law, 1993;Megreya et al, 2011) with a reversed effect occasionally reported for male faces (male observers better than female observers; see e.g., McKelvie et al, 1993).…”
Section: Sex Differencesmentioning
confidence: 99%