2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.039
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Significance of sniffing pattern during the acquisition of an olfactory discrimination task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(93 reference statements)
2
19
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This outcome is consistent with a need to sample odors longer during the reversal learning to maintain optimal performance accuracy (as the same level as the other groups), and this extended sampling is no longer needed when they are expert in the task, when the task becomes easy. In behaving rats performing a two-alternative odor discrimination task, the rats usually sniff around 6-8Hz (sniff cycles: 125-166ms) during the odor sampling period Lefevre et al 2016;Kepecs et al 2007). The increase of sampling duration observed here is sufficient to allow the rat to add another sniff.…”
Section: Relationship Between Sampling Duration and Performance: Speementioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This outcome is consistent with a need to sample odors longer during the reversal learning to maintain optimal performance accuracy (as the same level as the other groups), and this extended sampling is no longer needed when they are expert in the task, when the task becomes easy. In behaving rats performing a two-alternative odor discrimination task, the rats usually sniff around 6-8Hz (sniff cycles: 125-166ms) during the odor sampling period Lefevre et al 2016;Kepecs et al 2007). The increase of sampling duration observed here is sufficient to allow the rat to add another sniff.…”
Section: Relationship Between Sampling Duration and Performance: Speementioning
confidence: 81%
“…At the opposite, keeping short discrimination times can lead to a drop in performance in difficult odor discrimination (Uchida and Mainen 2003). Sampling durations can also vary during the acquisition of the two alternative odor discrimination task and are longer in 'good' vs. 'poor learners' (Lefevre et al 2016;Lovitz et al 2012). Sampling duration, response accuracy and difficulty of the tasks are thus intermingled.…”
Section: Relationship Between Sampling Duration and Performance: Speementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Respiratory activity was recorded by using whole-body plethysmography. Plethysmograph (EMKA Technologies, France) was previously described in detail 50,51 .…”
Section: Lfp and Respiration Recordingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Respiratory signals were acquired and extracted using previously described method 51 allowing accurate measurements of different respiratory parameters in behaving animals. The detection of the respiratory cycles was achieved using an enhanced version of the algorithm described in a previous study 55 .…”
Section: Respiratory Signalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is now clear that the dynamics of odor sampling during the first few sniffs taken by rodents during an odor identification task can provide enough information to allow odor identification and discrimination [1][2][3]. The importance of odor sampling by active sniffing for olfactory perception by humans and rodents has been known for more than three decades [4][5][6][7][8]. Active sniffing or odor sampling strategies also dramatically alter synaptic interactions in the olfactory bulb, the first central synaptic processing site for odor-elicited sensory input [9,10].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%