2017
DOI: 10.1140/epjb/e2017-80337-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Signs of universality in the structure of culture

Abstract: Abstract. Understanding the dynamics of opinions, preferences and of culture as whole requires more use of empirical data than has been done so far. It is clear that an important role in driving this dynamics is played by social influence, which is the essential ingredient of many quantitative models. Such models require that all traits are fixed when specifying the "initial cultural state". Typically, this initial state is randomly generated, from a uniform distribution over the set of possible combinations o… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If the distance d ij between their cultural vectors is smaller than ω and if these vectors are different with respect to at least one feature, then an interaction successfully occurs with probability 1−d ij : one of the agents switches its trait to match the trait of the other agent, with respect to one of the features that differentiates between them. This is exactly the model used in [15,17,18] and partly in [16]. As anticipated in sections 1 and 2, this model converges to a random final, absorbing state, one that consists of groups (cultural domains) of internally identical and externally non-interacting cultural vectors-distances within such groups are zero, while distances across are larger or equal to ω, as illustrated at the bottom of figure 1.…”
Section: Cultural Dynamics and Partition-specific Quantitiesmentioning
confidence: 70%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…If the distance d ij between their cultural vectors is smaller than ω and if these vectors are different with respect to at least one feature, then an interaction successfully occurs with probability 1−d ij : one of the agents switches its trait to match the trait of the other agent, with respect to one of the features that differentiates between them. This is exactly the model used in [15,17,18] and partly in [16]. As anticipated in sections 1 and 2, this model converges to a random final, absorbing state, one that consists of groups (cultural domains) of internally identical and externally non-interacting cultural vectors-distances within such groups are zero, while distances across are larger or equal to ω, as illustrated at the bottom of figure 1.…”
Section: Cultural Dynamics and Partition-specific Quantitiesmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…In practice, an ultrametric representation can be produced as the output of a hierarchical clustering algorithm applied to a matrix of pairwise distances between objects [19]. For the purpose of this work, these objects are the cultural vectors, whose pairwise cultural distances are computed in the same manner as in [15][16][17][18], based on a combination between the Hamming distance and the Manhattan distance, which are used in association with nominal and ordinal cultural featues, respectively-also see equation (A2) and the associated description for more details. The following explanations concerning ultrametricity are mostly restricted to cultural vectors, although many of the concepts have a wide range of applicability.…”
Section: Ultrametricity and Culturementioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations