2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Size averaging of irrelevant stimuli cannot be prevented

Abstract: Research suggests that subjects can compute the mean size of two sets of interspersed objects concurrently, but that doing so incurs a cost of dividing attention across the two sets. Alternatively, costs may arise from failing to exclude irrelevant items from the calculation of mean size. Here, we examined whether attention can be selectively deployed to prevent the inclusion of items from an irrelevant, concurrently displayed set in the computation of the relevant set's mean size. The results suggest that mea… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
77
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(88 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
7
77
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This fits with data suggesting that during the process of ensemble perception the visual system parses arrays of individual objects into groups, suggesting that the group as a whole is a target for property-attribution (e.g. Oriet and Brand 2013). Although one might worry that an ensemble isn't best described as an object, we should remember that in this context talk of 'objects' is rather loose.…”
Section: Epistemic Justification and The Perception Of Ensemble Propesupporting
confidence: 80%
“…This fits with data suggesting that during the process of ensemble perception the visual system parses arrays of individual objects into groups, suggesting that the group as a whole is a target for property-attribution (e.g. Oriet and Brand 2013). Although one might worry that an ensemble isn't best described as an object, we should remember that in this context talk of 'objects' is rather loose.…”
Section: Epistemic Justification and The Perception Of Ensemble Propesupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Our understanding of color categories and their effect on cognition may also be improved from investigating this issue. Since ensemble perception is now understood to operate under distributed attention [3], in the absence of serial processing [17], and compulsorily [34,35], if linguistic color categories have an effect on the encoding of a briefly presented colorful ensemble, it will provide further evidence for the depth of the effect of color categories on the perceptual or cognitive representation of color [24,26,27,29,[36][37][38][39][40][41][42][43].…”
Section: Categories and Ensemble Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This seemingly fundamental process of perceptual averaging is pervasive in visual perception across task demands (e.g., Corbett et al, 2012; Oriet and Brand, 2013), focused attentional constraints (e.g., Chong and Treisman, 2005), and spatial reference frames (Corbett and Melcher, 2014a), and does not rely on precise representations of individual objects (e.g., Parkes et al, 2001; Choo and Franconeri, 2010; Corbett and Oriet, 2011). Average representations of size (Corbett et al, 2012), motion direction (e.g., Anstis et al, 1998), orientation (e.g., Gibson and Radner, 1937), texture density (e.g., Durgin, 1995), and numerosity (Burr and Ross, 2008) also undergo perceptual adaptation, a process by which the extended presentation of a stimulus biases the perception of subsequently presented stimuli in opposite directions along fundamental dimensions of the adapting stimulus.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%