2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Skilled readers’ sensitivity to meaningful regularities in English writing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

7
56
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
7
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In many Indo-European languages with concatenative morphology, sound-to-meaning mappings of morphologically complex words are typically less consistent than spelling-tomeaning mappings (e.g., Berg & Aronoff, 2017;Berg, Buchmann, Dybiec, & Fuhrhop, 2014;Rastle, 2018;Ulicheva, Harvey, Aronoff, & Rastle, 2018). For instance, in spoken English, the past tense is usually denoted by the allomorphs /əd/, /d/, or /t/ depending on surrounding context (e.g., busted, snored, kicked), whereas in written English, the corresponding phonemic sequences are always spelled ed (e.g., Carney, 1994;Desrochers, Manolitsis, Gaudreau, & Georgiou, 2017;Rastle, 2018).…”
Section: Morphological Processing Across Modalitiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many Indo-European languages with concatenative morphology, sound-to-meaning mappings of morphologically complex words are typically less consistent than spelling-tomeaning mappings (e.g., Berg & Aronoff, 2017;Berg, Buchmann, Dybiec, & Fuhrhop, 2014;Rastle, 2018;Ulicheva, Harvey, Aronoff, & Rastle, 2018). For instance, in spoken English, the past tense is usually denoted by the allomorphs /əd/, /d/, or /t/ depending on surrounding context (e.g., busted, snored, kicked), whereas in written English, the corresponding phonemic sequences are always spelled ed (e.g., Carney, 1994;Desrochers, Manolitsis, Gaudreau, & Georgiou, 2017;Rastle, 2018).…”
Section: Morphological Processing Across Modalitiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, it is important to look at more implicit rules that are less likely to be addressed explicitly in the spelling acquisition process: Morpho-graphic spelling of derivational suffixes in adults who have fully acquired the English spelling system. This is what Ulicheva et al (2020) did, testing whether skilled readers make use of morpho-graphic spelling regularities to induce the grammatical category of novel words, both in comprehension and production. In a categorisation task, they showed that participants were more likely to classify novel words with suffixes that are diagnostic of adjectives (e.g., -able, -ous and -less in gufable, raxous and jevless) as such, while novel words with nominal suffixes (e.g., -ment, -an and -er in vixment, dofan and jumer) were rather marked as representing nouns, suggesting that skilled readers' comprehension of novel words is aided by morphological information encoded in affixes with highly reliable spellings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…(4) In order to increase power, only one affix was investigated, namely -ous. This affix was selected because it is highly indicative of adjectivehood (see Berg and Aronoff 2017) and yielded the clearest differentiation between congruent (ADJ) and incongruent (N) contexts in Ulicheva et al (2020).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent computational work demonstrates that this strong relationship between suffix spelling and grammatical class is a general principle of English suffixation (Ulicheva, Harvey, Aronoff, & Rastle, 2018). Ulicheva et al (2018) studied the relationship between suffix spelling and grammatical class for more than 150 English suffixes; this relationship is visualised in Figure 1 for two suffixes. The first example shows that there are many possible spellings of the word-final sound sequence /lәs/ (e.g., [-lace], [-less], [-liss]).…”
Section: The Information In English Writingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, I would argue that the closer a writing system gets to a one-to-one mapping between spelling and sound, the less scope there is for highly reliable morphological cues to arise in the spelling, unless particular sound sequences themselves are reserved to communicate meaningful morphological information. However, this proposal is speculative; moving forward towards a more general understanding of how writing systems balance spelling-sound and spelling-meaning information will require much more detailed analyses of contrasting writing systems along the lines of those undertaken by Ulicheva et al (2018). More broadly, it seems almost inconceivable that the trajectory of reading acquisition, and the nature of representations characterising skilled reading, would not be substantially influenced by the nature of the information being learned (see also Frost, 2012).…”
Section: The Information In English Writingmentioning
confidence: 99%