2016
DOI: 10.1007/s00213-016-4221-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Smells like inhibition: The effects of olfactory and visual alcohol cues on inhibitory control

Abstract: RationaleHow the smell of alcohol impacts alcohol-related thoughts and behaviours is unclear, though it is well-documented that alcohol-related stimuli and environments may trigger these.ObjectivesThe current study, therefore, aimed to investigate the priming effects of both visual and olfactory alcohol cues on inhibitory control.MethodForty individuals (M age = 23.65, SD = 6.52) completed a go/no-go association task (GNAT) which measured reaction times, response accuracy and false alarm rates whilst being exp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
22
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
6
22
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The participants’ mean positive and negative outcome expectances at baseline (before testing) were 82.86 ( SD = 8.58) and 47.45 ( SD = 8.34), respectively, and their average AUDIT score was 16.02 ( SD = 3.22), which is higher than the cut‐off for clinical assessment (scores of 8 or more are deemed to indicate hazardous or harmful alcohol use; Babor, Higgins‐Biddle, Saunders, and Monteiro, ; Saunders et al, ). However, such scores are similar to other research using predominantly U.K. student samples (Clarke, Field, & Rose, ; Monk, Sunley, Qureshi, & Heim, ; Moss, Spada, et al, ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 88%
“…The participants’ mean positive and negative outcome expectances at baseline (before testing) were 82.86 ( SD = 8.58) and 47.45 ( SD = 8.34), respectively, and their average AUDIT score was 16.02 ( SD = 3.22), which is higher than the cut‐off for clinical assessment (scores of 8 or more are deemed to indicate hazardous or harmful alcohol use; Babor, Higgins‐Biddle, Saunders, and Monteiro, ; Saunders et al, ). However, such scores are similar to other research using predominantly U.K. student samples (Clarke, Field, & Rose, ; Monk, Sunley, Qureshi, & Heim, ; Moss, Spada, et al, ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 88%
“…As such, it may be that findings from the current placebo condition are reflective of alcohol expectancies (Martin & Sayette, 1993). Indeed, the mere presence of alcohol-related olfactory cues (as would be the case in the current placebo condition) has been shown to hinder participants' ability to inhibit their behaviour (Monk, Sunley, Qureshi, & Heim, 2016). Future research may therefore benefit from the additional inclusion of a pure control group (for example, the use of a soft drink where there is no suggestion of alcohol consumption).…”
Section: Preliminarymentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Jones y Field, 2013) han de tener en cuenta el efecto desinhibidor variable de diferentes modalidades de estímulos relacionados con el alcohol dirigidos a sentidos diferentes (c.f. Monk, Sunley, Qureshi, y Heim, 2016). Esto pueden tener implicaciones importantes para la implementación efectiva de este tipo de entrenamiento en el mundo real, donde las personas están rodeadas de una variedad de estímulos visuales y auditivos asociados al alcohol.…”
Section: Discussionunclassified