The Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression 2007
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511816840.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social‐Cognitive Processes in the Development of Antisocial and Violent Behavior

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 103 publications
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, as Lemerise and Arsenio (2000) point out, individual differences in emotionality and emotion regulation skills will also influence the goals children choose. For example, depending on how well children can regulate strong emotions, feelings of anger may make some children feel overwhelmed to the point that they choose an avoidant goal to modulate this strong feeling, whereas in aggressive children, anger may prime revenge or dominance goals (Lemerise et al 2006;Pettit and Mize 2007). Children with better emotion regulation skills may find it easier to maintain prosocial goals in the face of such a challenge (Lemerise et al 2006).…”
Section: Goal Clarificationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, as Lemerise and Arsenio (2000) point out, individual differences in emotionality and emotion regulation skills will also influence the goals children choose. For example, depending on how well children can regulate strong emotions, feelings of anger may make some children feel overwhelmed to the point that they choose an avoidant goal to modulate this strong feeling, whereas in aggressive children, anger may prime revenge or dominance goals (Lemerise et al 2006;Pettit and Mize 2007). Children with better emotion regulation skills may find it easier to maintain prosocial goals in the face of such a challenge (Lemerise et al 2006).…”
Section: Goal Clarificationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Three decades of research on social information processing (SIP) models of aggression and social competence have yielded reliable individual differences in children's social cognitive patterns that are associated with both aggression and socially competent behavior (see e.g., Crick and Dodge 1994;Mize and Pettit 2008;Pettit and Mize 2007 for reviews). However, very few studies within this tradition have examined the contributions of emotion processes to SIP, despite theoretical and empirical work which links individual differences in social competence and/or aggression to aspects of emotionality, arousal, and emotion regulation (e.g., Berkowitz 1962Berkowitz , 1989DiLiberto et al 2002;Dodge 1985Dodge , 1991Eisenberg and Fabes 1992;Eisenberg et al 1996;Graham et al 1992).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The argument, of course, was that emotional processes (e.g., emotion regulation) need be more central to a model of SIP that attempts to comprehensively account for individual differences in the development of youths' social competence. The importance of understanding emotion's role in the development of social cognition and antisocial behavior has been emphasized elsewhere, as well (e.g., Fontaine 2008a; Pettit and Mize 2007).…”
Section: Sip and Emotionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To date, no study has rigorously explored this relation, despite considerable research that supports the general SIP hypothesis that these (and other) social cognitive processes play critical roles in antisocial behavioral change across time. The question that guided the current study is whether RED is so important a set of processes by the time youths are developing through adolescence, and making considerable gains in executive function (Pettit and Mize 2007), that behavioral Some earlier studies that examined the relation between attributional and response evaluation processes in children would suggest that these two SIP components play unique roles and together have an additive effect, as opposed to a mediational relation. For example, Dodge et al (1986) conducted a pair of studies that examined the relations between children's social-information processing patterns and their interpersonal behavior in nonclinical and clinical samples.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This finding has been replicated across varied populations, contexts, and developmental periods (Orobio de Castro et al 2002). The phenomenon has been termed "hostile attributional bias" (Dodge 1980) to denote the aggressive child's interpreting difficulty, though more recently the term "hostile attributional style" (HAS; e.g., Dodge 2006) has been used.Individual differences in children's social cognitive development suggest that some children do not develop the cognitive skills to correctly interpret others' intentions until later childhood and adolescence (Pettit and Mize 2007). Adolescents' use of their evaluative skills appears to become especially important in the later stages of the SIP model (e.g., see Lansford et al 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%