2020
DOI: 10.1007/s42761-020-00022-w
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social Context Influences the Acoustic Properties of Laughter

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
12
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
3
12
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One is the tension of the vocal fold adduction linked to physiological changes and vocal effort: Hypertension and hypotension can lead to irregular vocal fold vibration and nonlinear vocal phenomena (Anikin, 2020b; Fitch et al, 2002; Scherer, 1989). Furthermore, roughness, an overall measure of amplitude and frequency modulation, has previously been described for extremely intense vocalizations, such as screams (Arnal et al, 2015); as well as for vocalizations including unvoiced parts and a breathy voice quality (Wood, 2020), associated with lower emotional intensity (see Figures 3 and 4). A promising avenue for future work, for which the VIVAE materials are a suitable resource, is a more fine-grained analysis of nonlinear phenomena with regard to the perceived emotional intensity and the behavioral relevance of different types of ‘noise’ in such variably-intense vocalizations.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…One is the tension of the vocal fold adduction linked to physiological changes and vocal effort: Hypertension and hypotension can lead to irregular vocal fold vibration and nonlinear vocal phenomena (Anikin, 2020b; Fitch et al, 2002; Scherer, 1989). Furthermore, roughness, an overall measure of amplitude and frequency modulation, has previously been described for extremely intense vocalizations, such as screams (Arnal et al, 2015); as well as for vocalizations including unvoiced parts and a breathy voice quality (Wood, 2020), associated with lower emotional intensity (see Figures 3 and 4). A promising avenue for future work, for which the VIVAE materials are a suitable resource, is a more fine-grained analysis of nonlinear phenomena with regard to the perceived emotional intensity and the behavioral relevance of different types of ‘noise’ in such variably-intense vocalizations.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…At the same time, there is some leeway in the meaning of all human vocalizations. For example, laughs are typically associated with amusement and are therefore less ambiguous than screams and moans, but they can still express a range of social attitudes including malice, sarcasm and nervousness [ 24 , 41 , 42 ]. Thus, contextual cues are likely to enrich the meaning of most nonverbal displays, even those that are not inherently ambiguous, a hypothesis that now needs to be empirically tested.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings are in line with research showing that nonverbal vocalizations in general are a potent tool to communicate the sender’s affective state, such as different positive emotions 10 . Wood 31 found evidence that differences in acoustic parameters are related to differences in the affective context of laughter, i.e., laughter emitted during the perception of videoclips evoking reward (humorous laughter), affiliation (humorous and tenderness laughter), and dominance (derisive and ridiculing laughter) showed differences in acoustic parameters. Similarly, Tanaka & Campbell 32 have shown that in spontaneous conversations polite-formal laughs differ acoustically from mirthful laughs.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%