2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2014.09.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social dilemmas: When self-control benefits cooperation

Abstract: Individuals in a social dilemma may experience a self-control conflict between urges to act selfishly and better judgment to cooperate. Pairing a public goods game with a subtle framing technique, we test whether perception of self-control conflict strengthens the association between self-control and cooperation. Consistent with our hypothesis, cooperative behavior is positively associated with self-control in the treatment that raised the relative likelihood of perceiving conflict, but not associated with sel… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

4
40
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 85 publications
4
40
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Experimental evidence as well as theoretical considerations also suggest that both of these attributes could drive behavior in PGGs. Martinsson et al (2014) demonstrate that subjects with higher self-control cooperate more. Similarly Fehr and Leibbrandt (2011) show how patience assessed by a lab measure predicts cooperative behavior in the field.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Experimental evidence as well as theoretical considerations also suggest that both of these attributes could drive behavior in PGGs. Martinsson et al (2014) demonstrate that subjects with higher self-control cooperate more. Similarly Fehr and Leibbrandt (2011) show how patience assessed by a lab measure predicts cooperative behavior in the field.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Next to procedural differences, the social choice designs used in these studies differed in the degree of familiarity, or social closeness, between the participants and their interaction partners. For example, in some studies, the interaction partner was anonymous, whereas in others, he/she was introduced to the participants, and yet in other studies, the interaction partners were actual acquaintances or co-students [18]. Interestingly, whether the interaction partner was anonymous or not appeared to determine the sign of the cognitive load effects on generosity: when the interaction partner was an anonymous stranger, subjects often became more generous under cognitive load conditions [9,11], but when the interaction partner was familiar to the subject, e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, whether the interaction partner was anonymous or not appeared to determine the sign of the cognitive load effects on generosity: when the interaction partner was an anonymous stranger, subjects often became more generous under cognitive load conditions [9,11], but when the interaction partner was familiar to the subject, e.g. the interaction partner was a co-student or introduced before, he/she became more selfish [16,18]. When familiarity was not well controlled, any putative effects of cognitive load on social preferences might have been obscured by the uncontrolled variability in anonymity.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is this joint resultego depletion affects pro-social behavior without affecting pro-social concerns-that represents the key contribution of their paper. We expand on the significance of their results by relating the pattern to recent theorizing about pro-social behavior and the two-stage model of self-control, which distinguishes between identifying self-control conflict and exercising restraint (Martinsson, Myrseth, & Wollbrant, 2012;Kocher, Martinsson, Myrseth, & Wollbrant, 2013;Martinsson, Myrseth, & Wollbrant, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%