2021
DOI: 10.1086/714992
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Socioeconomic Status and Inequalities in Children’s IQ and Economic Preferences

Abstract: This paper explores inequalities in IQ and economic preferences between children from high and low socio-economic status (SES) families. We document that children from high SES families are more intelligent, patient and altruistic, as well as less likely to be risk-seeking. To understand the underlying causes and mechanisms, we propose a framework of how parental investments as well as maternal IQ and economic preferences influence a child's IQ and preferences. Within this framework, we allow SES to influence … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
42
1

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 143 publications
1
42
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the home environment, high-SES parents spend more time reading with and to their children which likely stimulates cognitive growth and learning (e.g., Myrberg & Rosén, 2009). Moreover, children whose parents have higher SES do generally develop higher cognitive ability levels which are then transmitted to their children through both genes and the environment (Engelhardt et al, 2018;Falk et al, 2021;Turkheimer et al, 2003). Conversely, the lower degree of nurture and support among lower-SES children can affect the cognitive ability levels, and hence outputs in for example school tests, negatively (Flynn, 2012;Sackett et al, 2009;Turkheimer et al, 2003).…”
Section: The Influence Of Ses Cognitive Ability and Non-cognitive Abi...mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In the home environment, high-SES parents spend more time reading with and to their children which likely stimulates cognitive growth and learning (e.g., Myrberg & Rosén, 2009). Moreover, children whose parents have higher SES do generally develop higher cognitive ability levels which are then transmitted to their children through both genes and the environment (Engelhardt et al, 2018;Falk et al, 2021;Turkheimer et al, 2003). Conversely, the lower degree of nurture and support among lower-SES children can affect the cognitive ability levels, and hence outputs in for example school tests, negatively (Flynn, 2012;Sackett et al, 2009;Turkheimer et al, 2003).…”
Section: The Influence Of Ses Cognitive Ability and Non-cognitive Abi...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are strong theoretical and empirical reasons to assert that school achievement such as grades is influenced by SES, cognitive ability and non-cognitive abilities (e.g., Duckworth & Seligman, 2005;Falk et al, 2021). There are also strong reasons to believe that SES influences cognitive ability (Flynn, 2012;Myrberg & Rosén, 2009;Turkheimer et al, 2003), and that non-cognitive ability affects cognitive ability (Borghans et al, 2008;Heckman & Kautz, 2014).…”
Section: The Influence Of Ses Cognitive Ability and Non-cognitive Abi...mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the PISA data set, this variable is coded as BOOKS. Because parents appear to play a pivotal role for their children's academic achievement (e.g., Falk et al, 2021), variables that only in part cover socioeconomic background but nonetheless reflect the possible influence of parents were included, specifically SUPPORT1-4 (e.g., 1 My parents support me). All these were measured on a Likert scale.…”
Section: Independent Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Complementing children’s data with a survey among their parents allows studying the role of family background for the early development of grit. The family environment and parents’ characteristics have a large influence on various cognitive and non-cognitive skills 21 25 , but the relation of family background to the formation of grit in early childhood is still underexplored. So, in sum our paper is the first to present evidence on the development of grit in children as young as 3 to 6 years old, is able to present both a composite score of grit that captures many facets that are associated with grit, but at the same time can present evidence for the single components separately, and adds parental background and survey data to enrich our understanding of whether and to which extent grit develops in early age.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%