2010
DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arq143
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sophistication and simplicity: conventional communication in a rudimentary system

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
39
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The average call duration and rate vary among individuals, but organization rules of vocal communication remain the same. The sensitivity of barn owl nestlings to the partner's change in duration and rate in the preceding vocal sequence is an additional element showing that animals perceive complex vocal patterns (Rek & Osiejuk, 2010). Some species organize their vocal units following syntactic rules (ten Cate & Okanoya, 2012;Ouattara, Lemasson, & Zuberbuhler, 2009); the organization found in barn owls can be seen as a precursor of syntactic rules without any differentiated vocal units (Rek & Osiejuk, 2010).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The average call duration and rate vary among individuals, but organization rules of vocal communication remain the same. The sensitivity of barn owl nestlings to the partner's change in duration and rate in the preceding vocal sequence is an additional element showing that animals perceive complex vocal patterns (Rek & Osiejuk, 2010). Some species organize their vocal units following syntactic rules (ten Cate & Okanoya, 2012;Ouattara, Lemasson, & Zuberbuhler, 2009); the organization found in barn owls can be seen as a precursor of syntactic rules without any differentiated vocal units (Rek & Osiejuk, 2010).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We found that Corncrake males increased the rhythm of calling and performed fewer syllables per 1 min during the playback experiment than before the playback. A change to a more intermittent level of rhythm signals a more aggressive motivation of the sender (Ręk and Osiejuk 2010). In contrast, a decrease in the number of calls during aggressive interactions can be explained in two different ways.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…SYL2) (where SYL1 is the duration of the first syllable, SYL2 the duration of the second syllable, INT1 the duration of the withincall interval, and INT2 the duration of the between-call interval) and reflects the monotonous (low value of RHYTHM) or intermittent (high value of RHYTHM) character of calling, which can be detected easily by the human ear. Moreover, males respond more aggressively to the playback of an intermittent rhythm than to that of a monotonous rhythm, and males with intermittent calls are more aggressive (Ręk and Osiejuk 2010). To overcome this problem, we controlled rhythm in our experiment.…”
Section: Preparation Of Call Stimuli and Experimental Protocolmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, song rhythm and its changes may be an efficient way of conventional signalling in species with simple vocalizations (Ręk and Osiejuk, 2010). However, other possible factors limiting the syllable rate in chiffchaff, such as constraints imposed by syrinx and oropharyngeal …”
Section: Syllable Rate and Respiratory Demands In Chiffchaffsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A physical attack during territorial encounters is considered as the highest level of aggressive behaviour (e.g., Dunn et al, 2004;Petrusková et al, 2007;Searcy et al, 2006), and conventional signalling allows avoiding such risky behaviour (Hurd and Enquist, 1998;Ręk and Osiejuk, 2010). Recently, particular attention has been paid to soft vocalizations that predict attack in various species (Ballentine et al, 2008;Ręk and Osiejuk, 2011;Searcy et al, 2006).…”
Section: Syllable Rate As a Predictor Of Attackmentioning
confidence: 99%