Target set size refers to the number of preexisting connections a studied word has to closely related concepts in long-term memory. The purpose of this investigation was to determine whether target set size influences feeling-of-knowing (FOK) ratings. The results showed that ratings were higher for targets connected to smaller sets, as compared with those connected to larger sets. Comparable effects were obtained with the use of different encoding strategies, including concreteness classifications and vowel naming, and with both meaningfully and phonologically related test cues. These findings indicate that FOKs are sensitive to competition between concepts linked to the target and that this sensitivity is independent of encoding strategy and type of test cue. Response time measures indicated that FOKs are sensitive to competition that arises when concepts are activated in parallel, whereas recall is more sensitive to competition that arises during sampling associated with search. Implications for various cue-based and target-based explanations of FOK effects are discussed.When people are unable to remember previously learned information they nevertheless can have a sense of whether they will be able to remember it later on (see, e.g., Blake, 1973;Eysenck, 1979;Hart, 1967; Leonesio & T. O. Nelson, 1990;Metcalfe, 1993; T. O. Nelson, Gerler, & Narens, 1984;Schacter, 1983; Schreiber & D. L. Nelson, in press;Schwartz & Metcalfe, 1992). A particularly vivid example ofthis phenomenon is the case of a person who has a tipof-the-tongue (TOT) experience. In this special case, a person is unable to recall the sought-after target information but nevertheless has a high feeling of knowing (FOK) for it (for a review, see Brown, 1991). On what kinds of factors are these feelings based, given that the targeted information is not accessible explicitly? Although the TOT example is a useful illustration, feelings of knowing are not limited to this special case. They represent a more general phenomenon, most likely occurring whenever people receive a memory prompt (Koriat, 1993;Reder & Ritter, 1992). Essentially they are predictions offuture memory performance, and they may play an important functional role in memory. Various researchers have proposed that such predictions are used to regulate the flow of processing during retrieval. For example, given a moderate to high FOK computed during the early stages of retrieval, a person may decide to initiate a more extensive search of memory. Given a very low FOK, a search may not be iniSome ofthe data reported in this article also were reported at the 35th annual meeting ofthe Psychonomic Society, November 1994, St. Louis. Correspondence should be addressed to T. Schreiber, Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, 426 Fraser Hall, Lawrence, KS 66045 (e-mail: schreibe@kuhub.cc.ukans.edu).-Accepted by previous editor, Geoffrey R. Loftus tiated at all (Reder, 1988;Reder & Ritter, 1992). FOKs also may be used as a basis for deciding when search should be terminated (Koriat, 1993 Th...