2008
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.602
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial memory during progressive disorientation.

Abstract: Human spatial representations of object locations in a room-sized environment were probed for evidence that the object locations were encoded relative not just to the observer (egocentrically) but also to each other (allocentrically). Participants learned the locations of 4 objects and then were blindfolded and either (a) underwent a succession of 70° and 200° whole-body rotations or (b) were fully disoriented and then underwent a similar sequence of 70° and 200° rotations. After each rotation, participants po… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
18
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
3
18
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The conclusion that representations of self-to-object and object-to-object spatial relations coexisted is consistent with the findings in previous studies (Sargent et al, 2008; Waller & Hodgson, 2006). The interaction between layout geometry and learning position on the selection of reference objects (egocentrically or allocentrically) has not been reported before.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…The conclusion that representations of self-to-object and object-to-object spatial relations coexisted is consistent with the findings in previous studies (Sargent et al, 2008; Waller & Hodgson, 2006). The interaction between layout geometry and learning position on the selection of reference objects (egocentrically or allocentrically) has not been reported before.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…The pointer’s axis of rotation was offset 24.75 cm from that of the chair. Although this offset could require slight transformations of self-to-target directional responses, use of such an offset is common in the literature (e.g., Easton & Sholl, 1995; Holmes & Sholl, 2005; Sargent et al, 2008). Participants in the current study underwent pointer training so that they would become familiar with the offset.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sargent, Dopkins, Philbeck, and Modarrez (2008) searched for evidence that spatial representations are updated in both global and piecemeal fashion. Participants learned the location of four target objects in a room-sized environment from a fixed heading amid the array.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The disorientation effect when people learn an irregular layout while standing in the middle of it was replicated in several follow-up studies (Mou, McNamara, Rump, & Xiao, 2006; Waller & Hodgson, 2006; Xiao, Mou, & McNamara, 2009; Sargent, Dopkins, Philbeck, & Modarres, 2008; but see Holmes & Sholl, 2005). There are two alternative explanations of the disorientation effect.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%