1999
DOI: 10.1364/josaa.16.002112
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial-scale contribution to the detection of mirror symmetry in fractal noise

Abstract: We investigated how the detection of mirror symmetry depends on the distribution of contrast energy across spatial scales. Stimuli consisted of vertically symmetric noise patterns with fractal power spectra defined by 1/f beta slopes (-2 < or = beta < or = 5). While overall rms contrast remained fixed at 25%, symmetry-detection thresholds were obtained by corrupting the signal with variable amounts of noise with identical spectral characteristics. A first experiment measured thresholds as a function of spectra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
29
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Based on these results, they proposed that similar mechanisms might underlie the encoding of orientation and the encoding of symmetry. This accords with studies that demonstrate the simultaneous processing of symmetry at different spatial scales and for different orientation content, suggesting that simple cortical filters such as those found in V1 could subserve symmetry detection [71,86,87,[107][108][109].…”
Section: Neural Implementationsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Based on these results, they proposed that similar mechanisms might underlie the encoding of orientation and the encoding of symmetry. This accords with studies that demonstrate the simultaneous processing of symmetry at different spatial scales and for different orientation content, suggesting that simple cortical filters such as those found in V1 could subserve symmetry detection [71,86,87,[107][108][109].…”
Section: Neural Implementationsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…This is because structural information at the object level becomes finer in spatial terms as the object moves farther away, gradually slipping below the acuity limit of the observer. Also, in natural images, low spatial frequencies have much higher contrast levels, with the energy dropping off as a 1/f n function, where f is frequency and n is an exponent between about 1.1 and 2.2 (see, e.g., Field & Brady, 1997;Rainville & Kingdom, 1999). This means that obscuration that reduces contrast-such as glare or fog-will tend to affect the higher frequencies more.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, there will almost certainly be some variability in the difficulty of recognizing different object classes when their images are filtered at any given cutoff. However, there do exist striking statistical regularities in the distribution of energy and information across spatial frequency in natural images overall (see, e.g., Field & Brady, 1997; Rainville & Kingdom, 1999), so it is unlikely that object recognition performance, relative to spatial filtering, will be completely idiosyncratic. That is, it seems likely that our results have some degree of generalizability to other object classes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The priming effect was defined by Δd', calculated by subtracting d' on random primes from d' on one-fold symmetrical primes. The priming effects were not significant, negative, and positive when the symmetry axes in the prime and target had the same orientation, nonorthogonal orientations, and orthogonal orientations, respectively (similar filtering techniques had been employed by, e.g., Dakin & Herbert, 1998;Julesz & Chang, 1979;Rainville & Kingdom, 1999. Each image was generated starting from randomly distributed Gaussian luminance (μ = 0, σ = 0.125).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%