2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.09.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatio-temporal dynamics of action-effect associations in oculomotor control

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, it seems that only a few learning instances might be sufficient to establish action-effect associations strong enough to trigger proactive effect-monitoring processes and corresponding anticipatory saccades. This matches findings by Riechelmann et al (2017). In their study, goal-directed left/right eye movements led to the upper/ lower light of a traffic light stimulus turning on.…”
supporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, it seems that only a few learning instances might be sufficient to establish action-effect associations strong enough to trigger proactive effect-monitoring processes and corresponding anticipatory saccades. This matches findings by Riechelmann et al (2017). In their study, goal-directed left/right eye movements led to the upper/ lower light of a traffic light stimulus turning on.…”
supporting
confidence: 83%
“…Interestingly, action-effect anticipation has also been studied in eye movements. However, in the majority of studies on action-effect anticipation in eye movements, saccadic responses were instructed and used as a substitute for manual responses to assess ideomotor influences on action selection (e.g., Herwig & Horstmann, 2011; Huestegge & Kreutzfeldt, 2012; Riechelmann et al, 2017). In opposition to this trend, Pfeuffer et al (2016) suggested that spontaneously occurring, uninstructed eye movements could carry additional information about action-effect anticipation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The above-reviewed binding evidence suggests that the socio-affective consequences of actions are coded in a generally similar way to non-social outcomes. Previous studies have shown saccade control can be guided by action-outcome effects, albeit in a non-social context (e.g Huestegge & Kreutzfeldt, 2012;Riechelmann, Pieczykolan, Horstmann, Herwig, & Huestegge, 2017). Relatedly, one eye-tracking study demonstrated that action-effect associations are made by the oculomotor system within a social context (Herwig & Hortsmann, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interaction of bottom-up and top-down processing is captured in many models of spatial attention and fixation distribution (e.g., Nuthmann, Smith, Engbert, & Henderson, 2010;Wolfe, Cave, & Franzel, 1989). In between this continuum from top-down to bottom-up control, other additional sources of influence have been assumed, including selection determined by previous selection targets (i.e., based on selection history), by reward contingencies associated with targets (i.e., reward history; see Awh, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2012;Hickey & van Zoest, 2012, for a review), or by other types of anticipation of effects associated with eye movements (Huestegge & Kreutzfeldt, 2012;Pfeuffer, Kiesel, & Huestegge, 2016;Riechelmann, Pieczykolan, Horstmann, Herwig, & Huestegge, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%