2022
DOI: 10.1002/ece3.9277
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatiotemporal patterns of male and female white‐tailed deer on a hunted landscape

Abstract: Resource selection in sexually dimorphic ungulates is at least partially explained by sex‐specific resource requirements and risk aversion strategies. Females generally spend more time in areas with less risk and abundant, high‐quality forage due to their smaller body size. However, demographically variable responses to risk are context dependent, and few have concurrently quantified male and female behavior within areas with the same resource base. We captured 111 (54 males, 57 females) adult white‐tailed dee… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To investigate these potential population and community effects, it is necessary to compare the impacts of human and nonhuman predation risk on prey antipredator behaviors (Clinchy et al, 2016; Crawford et al, 2022; Suraci, Frank, et al, 2019). Examples of antipredator behavioral responses are changes in vigilance (Creel et al, 2014; Laundre et al, 2001; Schuttler et al, 2017), foraging (Smith et al, 2017; Van Beeck Calkoen et al, 2022), movement (Courbin et al, 2015; Crawford et al, 2022; Gehr et al, 2018), and resource selection (Perry et al, 2020; Stewart et al, 2022; Suraci, Clinchy, et al, 2019). Although studies have compared the impacts of human versus nonhuman fear on vigilance and foraging using auditory playbacks and cameras (Crawford et al, 2022; Smith et al, 2017), and used GPS data to investigate the impacts of hunters on movement and resource selection (Collier et al, 2017; Gross et al, 2015; Perry et al, 2020; Stewart et al, 2022; Sullivan et al, 2018), few studies have compared the impacts of human and nonhuman predation risk on movement and resource selection of prey.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To investigate these potential population and community effects, it is necessary to compare the impacts of human and nonhuman predation risk on prey antipredator behaviors (Clinchy et al, 2016; Crawford et al, 2022; Suraci, Frank, et al, 2019). Examples of antipredator behavioral responses are changes in vigilance (Creel et al, 2014; Laundre et al, 2001; Schuttler et al, 2017), foraging (Smith et al, 2017; Van Beeck Calkoen et al, 2022), movement (Courbin et al, 2015; Crawford et al, 2022; Gehr et al, 2018), and resource selection (Perry et al, 2020; Stewart et al, 2022; Suraci, Clinchy, et al, 2019). Although studies have compared the impacts of human versus nonhuman fear on vigilance and foraging using auditory playbacks and cameras (Crawford et al, 2022; Smith et al, 2017), and used GPS data to investigate the impacts of hunters on movement and resource selection (Collier et al, 2017; Gross et al, 2015; Perry et al, 2020; Stewart et al, 2022; Sullivan et al, 2018), few studies have compared the impacts of human and nonhuman predation risk on movement and resource selection of prey.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, the risk allocation hypothesis suggests antipredator behavior depends on both immediate short‐ and long‐term risk (Lima & Bednekoff, 1999; Moll et al, 2017), wherein prey should exhibit strongest antipredator behavior during brief high‐risk situations within low background risk, and weakest antipredator behaviors during pulses of safety within high background risk (Creel et al, 2005; Lima & Bednekoff, 1999). While proactive and reactive responses to predation risk have both been documented to have population‐ and community‐level effects (Courbin et al, 2015; Fortin et al, 2005; Stewart et al, 2022; Sullivan et al, 2018), they are not mutually exclusive and are rarely considered simultaneously (Creel et al, 2014; Gehr et al, 2018). Investigating both proactive and reactive movements simultaneously allows one to test the risk allocation hypothesis from human‐ and nonhuman‐induced risk at multiple spatial and temporal scales.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Understanding the determinants of animal space use and habitat selection patterns implies taking into account circadian and annual cycles in animal behavior (e.g., Lagos et al, 2012;Stewart et al, 2022).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Understanding the determinants of animal space use and habitat selection patterns implies taking into account circadian and annual cycles in animal behavior (e.g., Lagos et al., 2012 ; Stewart et al., 2022 ). Temporal variations can affect resource consumption (Merkle et al., 2016 ), predation (Kittle et al., 2022 ), and, consequently, individual survival (Jessop et al., 2018 ; Larsen & Boutin, 1994 ) and reproduction (Fahrig, 2007 ; Robertson et al., 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%