2013
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1212417110
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Specular reflections and the estimation of shape from binocular disparity

Abstract: Binocular stereopsis is a powerful visual depth cue. To exploit it, the brain matches features from the two eyes' views and measures their interocular disparity. This works well for matte surfaces because disparities indicate true surface locations. However, specular (glossy) surfaces are problematic because highlights and reflections are displaced from the true surface in depth, leading to information that conflicts with other cues to 3D shape. Here, we address the question of how the visual system identifies… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
35
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
(43 reference statements)
0
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…and shape are influential in distinguishing materials by providing relevant visual information. Furthermore, as has been shown previously (Doerschner et al, 2011;Muryy, Welchman, Blake, & Fleming, 2013;Wendt, Faul, & Mausfeld, 2008), judgments of specular reflectance are affected by both binocular disparity and motion information. Many studies have analyzed this by presenting the two stimuli simultaneously.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…and shape are influential in distinguishing materials by providing relevant visual information. Furthermore, as has been shown previously (Doerschner et al, 2011;Muryy, Welchman, Blake, & Fleming, 2013;Wendt, Faul, & Mausfeld, 2008), judgments of specular reflectance are affected by both binocular disparity and motion information. Many studies have analyzed this by presenting the two stimuli simultaneously.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…textured object, we can think of this process as two eyes viewing a particular texture element of the surface of the object, with the brain charged with establishing correspondence between the retinal projections of the texture elements in the two images. In the specular case, we assume (like previous work on specular stereo; Muryy et al, 2013) that the definition of correspondence is essentially equivalent; namely, that the visual system seeks the image feature in one eye that matches the same image feature in the other eye. Thus, the basic task is the same (seeking correspondence between image features); however, in the specular case, these features originate from the reflection of the environment illumination map, rather than markings on surface itself.…”
Section: Virtual Illumination Mappingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, we show that the distribution of ortho-epipolar disparities is related to surface geometry, providing a constraint when estimating the curvature of the viewed object. Thereby we show that even though specular stereo signals do not support direct perceptual estimates of the physical shape of an object (Muryy, Welchman, Blake, & Fleming, 2013), specular disparity fields do carry information about the intimate relations between the viewing geometry and surface topography which could potentially be exploited by humans and artificial systems.…”
Section: Introduction and Goalsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Furthermore, sharing radiance also causes specular highlights to appear at the same convergence angle as the reflecting object's surface. This is in contrast with physical reality, where specular reflections appear at a depth different than that of the reflecting surface [Kirschmann 1895;Muryy et al 2013], an effect called highlight disparity. Again, this may cause observers to underestimate surface glossiness [Wendt et al 2010] and even reduce authenticity [Wendt et al 2008].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%