2017
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180084
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speech-in-speech perception and executive function involvement

Abstract: This present study investigated the link between speech-in-speech perception capacities and four executive function components: response suppression, inhibitory control, switching and working memory. We constructed a cross-modal semantic priming paradigm using a written target word and a spoken prime word, implemented in one of two concurrent auditory sentences (cocktail party situation). The prime and target were semantically related or unrelated. Participants had to perform a lexical decision task on visual … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Consequently, associations between perceptual performance and subcortical neural responses can also indirectly reflect the effect of the auditory cortex. In this regard, previous studies have shown that bilingual experience increases grey matter density in various cortex regions, including executive control regions [ 146 149 ], which are essential in challenging listening conditions [ 150 , 151 ]. Thus, it can be argued that bilinguals who tend to use more cortical resources in background noise may have more efficient backward processes, and consequently, their brainstem responses were found to be less susceptible to the effect of noise.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consequently, associations between perceptual performance and subcortical neural responses can also indirectly reflect the effect of the auditory cortex. In this regard, previous studies have shown that bilingual experience increases grey matter density in various cortex regions, including executive control regions [ 146 149 ], which are essential in challenging listening conditions [ 150 , 151 ]. Thus, it can be argued that bilinguals who tend to use more cortical resources in background noise may have more efficient backward processes, and consequently, their brainstem responses were found to be less susceptible to the effect of noise.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With respect to identifying which cognitive resources are allocated during tasks that incur listening effort, there is a relatively broad consensus that the primary cognitive resource in question tends to be working memory capacity and/or the allocation and use of attention (Heald & Nusbaum, ; Pichora‐Fuller et al, ; Wingfield, ). Following the discussion of Strauss and Francis (), attention may be the resource most directly applicable to respond to the demands of listening (Chun, Golomb, & Turk‐Browne, ; Song & Iverson, ; Strauss et al, ; Wild et al, ), though other related resources including working memory (Rudner et al, ; Wingfield, ) and executive function (Brännström, Karlsson, Waechter, & Kastberg, ; Perrone‐Bertolotti, Tassin, & Meunier, ; Ward et al, ) surely play a role. These theoretical constructs are closely related, and the precise nature of their relationship is the subject of a huge scientific literature (Awh, Vogel, & Oh, ; Cowan, ; Engle, ; Fougnie, ; McCabe, Roediger, McDaniel, Balota, & Hambrick, ), rendering it beyond the scope of this article to address in detail.…”
Section: Defining Listening Effortmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is a broad consensus that listeners faced with noise-related or speech-related listening challenges must allocate cognitive resources to overcome them (Pichora-Fuller et al, 2016 ; Shinn-Cunningham & Best, 2008 ; Van Engen & Peelle, 2014 ; Zekveld, Kramer, & Festen, 2011 ). Likely resources (broadly defined) include working memory capacity (Brännström, Karlsson, Waechter, & Kastberg, 2018 ; Ingvalson, Lansford, Fedorova, & Fernandez, 2017a , 2017b ; Pichora-Fuller et al, 2016 ; Rudner, Lunner, Behrens, Thorén, & Rönnberg, 2012 ; Wingfield, 2016 ), selective attention/executive control (Heald & Nusbaum, 2014 ; Song & Iverson, 2018 ; Strauss & Francis, 2017 ; Ward, Shen, Souza, & Grieco-Calub, 2017 ; Wild et al, 2012 ), processing speed (Ingvalson et al, 2017a ), and/or other executive functions such as inhibitory control and switching cost (Brännström et al, 2018 ; Ingvalson et al, 2017a , 2017b ; Perrone-Bertolotti et al, 2017 ). Thus, individual differences in cognitive capacity, perhaps especially selective attention and working memory, may underlie differences in listening performance both within and across contexts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%