2020
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-60074-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speech Intonation Induces Enhanced Face Perception in Infants

Abstract: Infants' preference for faces with direct compared to averted eye gaze, and for infant-directed over adult-directed speech, reflects early sensitivity to social communication. Here, we studied whether infant-directed speech (IDS), could affect the processing of a face with direct gaze in 4-month-olds. In a new ERP paradigm, the word 'hello' was uttered either in IDS or adult-direct speech (ADS) followed by an upright or inverted face. We show that the face-specific N290 ERP component was larger when faces were… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We operationalized this idea by modifying the order of events in our task: we presented the speech stimuli before the execution of the grasping action (and not after, as in Experiment 2). Infant-directed speech is an ostensive signal itself (Csibra, 2010;Sirri et al, 2020;Zangl & Mills, 2007) and could lead infants to expect subsequent actions to be communicative (Okumura et al, 2013a;Senju & OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science . Although the change of order between speech and action seems subtle, we predicted that it should be sufficient to influence the infants' take on the grasping action, because the initial ostensive signal could facilitate a communicative, and inhibit an instrumental, interpretation of the subsequent action (see also Baker et al, 2009).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We operationalized this idea by modifying the order of events in our task: we presented the speech stimuli before the execution of the grasping action (and not after, as in Experiment 2). Infant-directed speech is an ostensive signal itself (Csibra, 2010;Sirri et al, 2020;Zangl & Mills, 2007) and could lead infants to expect subsequent actions to be communicative (Okumura et al, 2013a;Senju & OPEN MIND: Discoveries in Cognitive Science . Although the change of order between speech and action seems subtle, we predicted that it should be sufficient to influence the infants' take on the grasping action, because the initial ostensive signal could facilitate a communicative, and inhibit an instrumental, interpretation of the subsequent action (see also Baker et al, 2009).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, we presented the speech stimuli before the execution of the grasping action (and not after, as in Experiment 2). Infantdirected speech intonation is an ostensive signal itself (Csibra, 2010;Sirri et al, 2020;Zangl & Mills, 2007) and could lead infants to expect subsequent actions to be communicative (Senju & Csibra, 2008;Okumura et al, 2013a). Although the change of order between speech and action seems subtle, we predicted that it should be sufficient to influence the infants' take on the grasping action, because the initial ostensive signal could facilitate a communicative, and inhibit an instrumental, interpretation of the subsequent action (see also, Baker et al, 2009).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Infants may exploit the prosodic layer of IDS for communicative intentions to gain access to its contents (i.e., informative intentions). IDS modulates electrophysiological responses to faces in 4-month-olds, perhaps because it generates communicative expectations (Sirri et al, 2020). It is interpreted as an ostensive cue by 5-6-month-olds, just as eye-gaze (Senju and Csibra, 2008;Parise and Csibra, 2013;Lloyd-Fox et al, 2015).…”
Section: Communicative Self-referentiality In the Speech Signalmentioning
confidence: 99%