2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.08.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spoken-word recognition in 2-year-olds: The tug of war between phonological and semantic activation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

8
56
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(64 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
8
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In prior work with adults, the presence of a related distractor slowed participants' search relative to baseline trials in which the distractors were unrelated to the target, as participants were more likely to fixate the related item while searching the array (Moores et al, ). Recent work using this paradigm showed similar evidence for children as young as 3 (Vales, Unger, & Fisher, ; see Chow et al, for related evidence with toddlers), suggesting that children co‐activated related items upon hearing the name of the target. However, because in those experiments the distractors did not systematically vary in strength, it remains an open question whether this co‐activation is modulated by the strength of the relation between the target and the related distractor.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In prior work with adults, the presence of a related distractor slowed participants' search relative to baseline trials in which the distractors were unrelated to the target, as participants were more likely to fixate the related item while searching the array (Moores et al, ). Recent work using this paradigm showed similar evidence for children as young as 3 (Vales, Unger, & Fisher, ; see Chow et al, for related evidence with toddlers), suggesting that children co‐activated related items upon hearing the name of the target. However, because in those experiments the distractors did not systematically vary in strength, it remains an open question whether this co‐activation is modulated by the strength of the relation between the target and the related distractor.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…For example, in priming studies, children's lexical decisions about a target word are modulated by a preceding semantically related word: School‐aged children respond faster on these primed trials relative to control trials (Simpson & Lorsbach, , ; Plaut & Booth, ; see also McCauley, Weil, & Sperber, ; Perraudin & Mounound, ). Related studies with infants and toddlers have shown that processing a target item (typically, its visual depiction; but see Willits et al, ) is facilitated by a preceding semantically related word (Arias‐Trejo & Plunkett, , ; Chow et al, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Multiple studies with adults have yielded evidence that semantic relatedness between targets and distractors influence visual search (Huettig & Altmann, 2005;Moores et al, 2003), and have used this phenomenon to investigate the relations that organize semantic knowledge (Mirman & Graziano, 2012;Mirman & Magnuson, 2009). According to recent research, similar effects of semantic distractors are evident in infants and young children (Bergelson & Aslin, 2017;Chow, Davies, & Plunkett, 2017;Vales & Fisher, 2019;Vales, Unger, & Fisher, 2017), highlighting the appropriateness of this approach for use with a broad developmental age range. Moreover, in the case of targets and related distractors whose labels co-occur, a study conducted by Yee, Overton, and Thompson-Schill (2009) provides evidence that looking at related distractors is likely driven by semantic links between the concepts that the labels denote.…”
Section: Present Studymentioning
confidence: 90%
“…First, 2‐year‐olds process familiar words faster if they also know many other words from the same category . In addition, eye‐tracking evidence suggests that when 2‐year‐olds hear a noun but do not see a picture of the word's referent (e.g., they hear the word banana but do not see a picture of a banana), they look to pictures of the word's taxonomic and associated neighbors (in this example, tomato or monkey ; ). Combined with the semantic priming studies cited earlier, this work suggests that lexical–semantic relations meaningfully influence the speed and pragmatics of early word comprehension, yet most researchers do not consider lexical–semantic structure when designing experiments or developing theories.…”
Section: The Effects Of Lexical–semantic Organization On Early Languamentioning
confidence: 99%