1978
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.4.2.136
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stimulus concreteness, response characteristics, and the recognition-recall method in paired-associate learning.

Abstract: Stimulus-word concreteness and type of response were varied in two studies using different versions of the recognition-recall method for paired-associate learning. With stimulus-recognition ratings, criterion shifts were suggested that were a joint function of stimulus concreteness and type of response (noun, number, or consonant-vowel-consonant [cvc] trigram). When criterion shifts were controlled with a forced-choice recognition procedure, stimulus concreteness was found to affect response recall strongly e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

1980
1980
1988
1988

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The decision to select randomly one item from the no-question passage segment to serve as a comparison rather than to use the average number of recalls from the noquestion segment was based on the consideration that a comparison of recall from a small class of items with recall from a larger class of items can be biased in favor of finding a superiority for the smaller class of items. Such a bias is expected on the assumption that the larger the class of items the greater the number of difficult items contained in that class (see Runquist, 1974;Wicker, Thorelli, & Saddler, 1978). Thus, using the average recall measure for the noquestion condition could have biased the comparison in favor of a superiority for the question conditions, in which recall involved only one specific item.…”
Section: Indirect Superordinate Recallmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The decision to select randomly one item from the no-question passage segment to serve as a comparison rather than to use the average number of recalls from the noquestion segment was based on the consideration that a comparison of recall from a small class of items with recall from a larger class of items can be biased in favor of finding a superiority for the smaller class of items. Such a bias is expected on the assumption that the larger the class of items the greater the number of difficult items contained in that class (see Runquist, 1974;Wicker, Thorelli, & Saddler, 1978). Thus, using the average recall measure for the noquestion condition could have biased the comparison in favor of a superiority for the question conditions, in which recall involved only one specific item.…”
Section: Indirect Superordinate Recallmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The decision to select randomly one test question as the basis for assessing recall, rather than to use the average number of test questions answered correctly, was based on the consideration that a comparison of recall from a small class of items with recall from a larger class of items can be biased in favor of finding a superiority for the smaller class of items. Such a bias is expected on the assumption that the larger the class of items the greater the number of difficult items contained in that class (see Runquist, 1974;Wicker, Thorelli, & Saddler, 1978). Consequently, using the average recall measure for the no pre-passage question condition could have biased the comparison in favor of the question conditions, in which overall the number of possible indirect recall opportunities was smaller.…”
Section: Design and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%