2016
DOI: 10.1111/eth.12463
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stress During Adolescence Shapes Performance in Adulthood: Context‐Dependent Effects on Foraging and Vigilance

Abstract: Exposure to chronic stress during adolescence can shape behaviour, cognition and physiology in adulthood, but the consequences of these longterm changes remain unclear. Prior studies reporting altered performance following exposure to stress in adolescence have generally interpreted lasting changes as impairments. However, we have recently shown that exposure to chronic unpredictable stress during adolescence (from postnatal days 30-70) can enhance performance in a context-dependent manner during a foraging ta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 96 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It could be an outcome of the stress response elicited by the social instability and crowding, as demonstrated by the reduction of food intake and the size alteration of the zona glomerulosa of the adrenal cortex. Social instability and crowding are recognized environmental stressors (Blanchard et al, 2001;Chaby et al, 2016), while stress is known to alter feeding responses in a bidirectional pattern, with both increases and decreases in intake (Maniam and Morris, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It could be an outcome of the stress response elicited by the social instability and crowding, as demonstrated by the reduction of food intake and the size alteration of the zona glomerulosa of the adrenal cortex. Social instability and crowding are recognized environmental stressors (Blanchard et al, 2001;Chaby et al, 2016), while stress is known to alter feeding responses in a bidirectional pattern, with both increases and decreases in intake (Maniam and Morris, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Animals were left in their home cages and exposed to a scent of a predator, fox urine (Tink’s Fox-P ® ), for 2 h. The scent was sprayed onto cotton balls and encased in a small ventilated plastic container, which was removed at the end of the 2 h (Chaby et al, 2015b ). Following scent exposure, rats were exposed to a recording of predatory feline calls for 20 min in their home cages (Chaby et al, 2015a , b , 2016 ). Finally, animals were placed in an open arena (92 × 92 × 63 cm) for 5 min with a hawk shaped kite hanging overhead, which was lowered towards the rats to mimic approach (Chaby et al, 2015b ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is critical in natural settings (Beauchamp, 2015) and occurs in the laboratory as well (Wallace et al, 2013;Yilmaz & Meister, 2013;Zambetti et al, 2019) Knowing how foraging computations are affected by interruptions may be critical to interpreting foraging decisions. Experiments that program the occurrence of startling events or introduce artificial predators during foraging could examine how these factors interact with foraging decisions (Amir et al, 2015;Chaby et al, 2016;Kim & Jung, 2018;Mobbs et al, 2018;Pellman & Kim, 2016;Walters et al, 2019;Zambetti et al, 2019). Recent years have also seen great advances in computational techniques for identifying statistical structure in animal behavior (Calhoun et al, 2019;Gris et al, 2017;Hsu & Yttri, 2021;McCullough & Goodhill, 2021;Wiltschko et al, 2015).…”
Section: How Is the Timing Of Multiple Behavioral Objectives Balanced?mentioning
confidence: 99%