1975
DOI: 10.2307/1165986
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Structure and Variation in Child Language

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
91
0
1

Year Published

1995
1995
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 316 publications
(96 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
4
91
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Children often produce verbs describing actions or physical motion such as throw or run before the second birthday (L. Bloom, Lightblown & Hood, 1975), and appear to understand them well (Huttenlocher, Smiley & Charney, 1983;Gentner, 1978). Think and know don't usually appear until the end of the third year of life (Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982;Shatz, Wellman & Silber, 1983) and are not well distinguished from one another in comprehension until around age four (Johnson & Maratsos, 1977;Moore, Bryant & Furrow, 1989;Moore & Furrow, 1991;Naigles, 2000).…”
Section: The Learning Trajectory For Mental Verbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children often produce verbs describing actions or physical motion such as throw or run before the second birthday (L. Bloom, Lightblown & Hood, 1975), and appear to understand them well (Huttenlocher, Smiley & Charney, 1983;Gentner, 1978). Think and know don't usually appear until the end of the third year of life (Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982;Shatz, Wellman & Silber, 1983) and are not well distinguished from one another in comprehension until around age four (Johnson & Maratsos, 1977;Moore, Bryant & Furrow, 1989;Moore & Furrow, 1991;Naigles, 2000).…”
Section: The Learning Trajectory For Mental Verbsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Identifying the underlying factors that cause children to produce such 'aberrant' structures has been one of the major challenges in language acquisition research. A number of factors have been suggested, including processing limitations (e.g., Pinker, 1984;Bloom, 1990), children's lack of knowledge of a particular word (e.g., Brown, 1973), immature syntactic structures (e.g., Hyams, 1986;Radford, 1990), and the non-verbal context of children's early interactions (e.g., Bloom, 1973;Brown, 1973;Greenfield and Smith, 1976). While it is most likely that all of these factors play a role in syntactic productions, in this article, I will be concerned with the particular contribution of social cognition to children's early utterances.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other languages, such as Inuktitut, it is also permissible to omit expressions in both subject and object positions, as in Milurpara, '(I) hit (him/her)'. It is well-documented that in the early stages of language development children omit subjects and objects at a higher rate and in different contexts from adults, whether or not the adult language syntactically allows this option (e.g., Greenfield and Smith, 1976;Hyams, 1986;Bloom, 1990;Gerken, 1991;Valian, 1991;Valian and Eisenberg, 1996;Allen, 2000;Grinstead, 2000;Skarabela and Allen, 2002). While much research on argument realization to date has been focused on the formal linguistic characteristics of argument omission in child language, virtually no attention or systematic investigation has been devoted to how this phenomenon might be influenced by children's socio-cognitive abilities, such as joint attention.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Overt subject use in child English has been extensively studied since the 1960s (e.g., Antinucci and Parisi 1973;Bates 1976;Bloom et al 1975aBloom et al , 1975bBrown and Fraser 1964;Guilfoyle 1984;Hyams 1986;Orfitelli and Hyams 2008;Valian 1991). As an illustration of the phenomenon, Valian (1991) shows that in a cross-sectional sample of US English-speaking children, the least grammatically developed, between MLU 1.5 and 2.0 (n = 6, age range 1;10-2;2), used overt subjects in roughly 70% of non-imperative sentences, while a sample of Italian-speaking children used overt subjects in roughly 30% of such utterances (n = 5, age range 2;0-2;5).…”
Section: Subject Occurrence In An Overt Subject Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As an illustration of the phenomenon, Valian (1991) shows that in a cross-sectional sample of US English-speaking children, the least grammatically developed, between MLU 1.5 and 2.0 (n = 6, age range 1;10-2;2), used overt subjects in roughly 70% of non-imperative sentences, while a sample of Italian-speaking children used overt subjects in roughly 30% of such utterances (n = 5, age range 2;0-2;5). Examples of such null subject sentences in child English include the following from Bloom et al (1975aBloom et al ( , 1975b and Brown (1973).…”
Section: Subject Occurrence In An Overt Subject Languagementioning
confidence: 99%