2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.06.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

28
453
4
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 467 publications
(487 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
28
453
4
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This has also been the case in other studies using global assessments of task-unrelated thoughts (Head & Helton, 2014). Researchers using more immediate thought probes have found an association between commission errors and reports of off-task thoughts (Smallwood et al, 2004), however, as McAvinue et al (2005 observed people are aware of their SART commission errors 99.1% of the time. If participants are probed immediately after a commission error in regards to whether they were task-focused or thinking about something else, their performance itself may influence their thought report (e.g., if a person makes an error and then is asked immediately what they were thinking about, they may conclude that because they made an error they must have been thinking about something other than the task).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…This has also been the case in other studies using global assessments of task-unrelated thoughts (Head & Helton, 2014). Researchers using more immediate thought probes have found an association between commission errors and reports of off-task thoughts (Smallwood et al, 2004), however, as McAvinue et al (2005 observed people are aware of their SART commission errors 99.1% of the time. If participants are probed immediately after a commission error in regards to whether they were task-focused or thinking about something else, their performance itself may influence their thought report (e.g., if a person makes an error and then is asked immediately what they were thinking about, they may conclude that because they made an error they must have been thinking about something other than the task).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…For example, do individuals with higher rates differ from those with lower rates in task-unrelated or task-relevant thoughts (cf. Smallwood et al, 2004)? As the items in all our questionnaires sample routine activities a variety of settings and contexts, receiving a high score implies a rather general trait, which might be taken to suggest a high rate of task-unrelated thought rather than ruminations about the specifics of task performance.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some evidence that the CFQ is correlated, inter alia, with attention-related errors comes from studies showing that the CFQ correlates with overt behavioral measures of attention (e.g., Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997;Tipper & Baylis, 1987). Nonetheless, although it is often employed as a measure of sustained attention (e.g., Robertson et al, 1997;Smallwood et al, 2004), it is clear that the CFQ measures proneness to more than just attention-related errors. Indeed, the items assessed by the CFQ were explicitly designed to be non-specific with regard to underlying cognitive processes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By collecting selfreports in an online fashion during scanning while keeping cognitive demands constant, and by examining differences in neural recruitment immediately before self-reports of being off versus on task, the present study was well positioned to overcome some of the limitations that have prevented previous research from drawing conclusive inferences about the role of the default network in mind wandering. To provide additional corroborative evidence for these self-reports, they were collected while participants performed a sustained attention to response task (SART) (12), during which performance errors have been linked to mind wandering (12)(13)(14)(15)(16)(17). This procedure allowed us to assess the convergence in brain activations between behavioral and subjective indices of mind wandering.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%