2017
DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000319
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Submentalizing or mentalizing in a Level 1 perspective-taking task: A cloak and goggles test.

Abstract: It has been proposed that humans possess an automatic system to represent mental states (‘implicit mentalizing’). The existence of an implicit mentalizing system has generated considerable debate however, centered on the ability of various experimental paradigms to demonstrate unambiguously such mentalizing. Evidence for implicit mentalizing has previously been provided by the ‘dot perspective task,’ where participants are slower to verify the number of dots they can see when an avatar can see a different numb… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

12
124
9
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(146 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
12
124
9
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our findings cannot be ascribed to decisional or response interference because a change in performance was only observed on Gabor-present Bself^trials (Bhits^), and response times did not mirror the effect of congruency on sensitivity. Indeed, in Experiment 3, response times were significantly speeded by congruent perspectives in both seeing and nonseeing conditions (thus tracking the low-level directional features of the avatar, consistent with recent findings; Cole, Atkinson, Le, & Smith, 2016;Conway, Lee, Ojaghi, Catmur, & Bird, 2017), whereas a boost in perceptual sensitivity was only observed in the seeing condition. Taken together, our findings suggest the avatar's viewpoint modulates early perceptual processing, thereby potentiating perception of stimuli for which others are also able to see and act upon (Otten, Seth, & Pinto, 2017;Teufel, Fletcher, & Davis, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Our findings cannot be ascribed to decisional or response interference because a change in performance was only observed on Gabor-present Bself^trials (Bhits^), and response times did not mirror the effect of congruency on sensitivity. Indeed, in Experiment 3, response times were significantly speeded by congruent perspectives in both seeing and nonseeing conditions (thus tracking the low-level directional features of the avatar, consistent with recent findings; Cole, Atkinson, Le, & Smith, 2016;Conway, Lee, Ojaghi, Catmur, & Bird, 2017), whereas a boost in perceptual sensitivity was only observed in the seeing condition. Taken together, our findings suggest the avatar's viewpoint modulates early perceptual processing, thereby potentiating perception of stimuli for which others are also able to see and act upon (Otten, Seth, & Pinto, 2017;Teufel, Fletcher, & Davis, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…We think this is less likely, given that in all conditions, the participant was not able to see the avatar's eyes, and was instead required to track (via the presence of the blindfold) whether they are in a seeing or nonseeing condition. To rule out bottom-up effects of the blindfold, future experiments could employ a belief induction manipulation in which the avatar wears either Bseeing^or Bnonseeingĉ oloured goggles (Conway et al, 2017). This ensures that the visual features of the avatars remain similar allowing changes in sensitivity to be directly attributed to the participant's belief about whether the goggles allow seeing or not (Heyes, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, there is ongoing debate about whether altercentric-interference effects in L1-VPT capture processes that are specifically social in nature (i.e., the "mentalizing" interpretation) or whether they reflect domain-general processes, such as attentional cueing (i.e., the "submentalizing" interpretation; Heyes, 2014). Experiments designed to test between these accounts have produced mixed results, with some findings supporting the mentalizing interpretation (e.g., Baker, Levin, & Saylor, 2016;Bukowski, Hietanen, & Samson, 2015;Furlanetto, Becchio, Samson, & Apperly, 2016;Gardner, Bileviciute, & Edmonds, 2018;Michael et al, 2018;Nielsen, Slade, Levy, & Holmes, 2015) and other findings supporting the sub-mentalizing interpretation (e.g., Cole, Atkinson, Le, & Smith, 2016;Conway, Lee, Ojaghi, Catmur, & Bird, 2017;Gardner, Hull, Taylor, & Edmonds, 2018;Langton, 2018;Santiesteban, Catmur, Hopkins, Bird, & Heyes, 2014;Wilson, Soranzo, & Bertamini, 2017). Although we suggest the PDP framework can help isolate avatar-perspective calculation as the construct of focal interest in indirect measures of L1-VPT, prior work indicates that the constructs captured by PDP parameters reflect a combination of domain-general and domain-specific processes (e.g., Payne, 2001Payne, , 2005.…”
Section: Implications Limitations and Future Research Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further research has focused on whether the perspective may not be a spontaneous phenomenon. If this is the case, then RAS would be only modulated by perceptual characteristics of the cue, while perspective taking would be due to top-down processes [18][19][20][21]. These authors noted that the human avatar employed by Samson et al [7] was unable to generate an attentional shift when the target discs were presented within 300 ms from the presentation of the cue.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%