2002
DOI: 10.1101/lm.44602
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Successive Olfactory Reversal Learning in Honeybees

Abstract: Honeybees Apis mellifera can associate an originally neutral odor with a reinforcement of sucrose solution. Forward pairings of odor and reinforcement enable the odor to release the proboscis extension reflex in consecutive tests. Bees can also be conditioned differentially: They learn to respond to a reinforced odor and not to a nonreinforced one. They can also learn to reverse their choice. Here we ask whether honeybees can learn successive olfactory differential conditioning tasks involving different overla… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
29
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
2
29
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, animals performing operant tasks for appetitive outcomes tend to repeat responses that were rewarded in immediately preceding trials (win-stay), whereas they tend to shift to alternative choices if preceding responses were not rewarded (lose-shift). These choice strategies have been reported in many studies spanning a wide array of tasks and species, including humans (Frank et al, 2007; Wang et al, 2014), nonhuman primates (Mishkin et al, 1962; Schusterman, 1962; Lee et al, 2004), rats (Evenden and Robbins, 1984; Skelin et al, 2014), mice (Means and Fernandez, 1992; Amodeo et al, 2012), pigeons (Rayburn-Reeves et al, 2013), and honeybees (Komischke et al, 2002). It is important to identify the neural mechanisms of these ubiquitous strategies to improve neurobiologically grounded theories of choice behavior.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…In particular, animals performing operant tasks for appetitive outcomes tend to repeat responses that were rewarded in immediately preceding trials (win-stay), whereas they tend to shift to alternative choices if preceding responses were not rewarded (lose-shift). These choice strategies have been reported in many studies spanning a wide array of tasks and species, including humans (Frank et al, 2007; Wang et al, 2014), nonhuman primates (Mishkin et al, 1962; Schusterman, 1962; Lee et al, 2004), rats (Evenden and Robbins, 1984; Skelin et al, 2014), mice (Means and Fernandez, 1992; Amodeo et al, 2012), pigeons (Rayburn-Reeves et al, 2013), and honeybees (Komischke et al, 2002). It is important to identify the neural mechanisms of these ubiquitous strategies to improve neurobiologically grounded theories of choice behavior.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…One of three odors-2-octanol, limonene, and peppermint-was used as a CS, and 30% sucrose solution was used as the unconditioned appetitive stimulus (US). The odors were delivered by an olfactometer (Galizia et al 1997;Komischke et al 2002). A continuous stream of air was blown over the bee's antennae, which was switched to a cartridge containing 4 mL of odor pipetted onto a half square inch filter paper, inside a 1-mL syringe.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Citral, geraniol, 2-hexanal, nonanone, 2-octanol, and limonene were used as odor CSs, referred to as A, B, and C in later sections. A computer-driven odor device, blowing a continuous stream of air over the antennae, was used to deliver the odors to the bees (Galizia et al 1997;Komischke et al 2002). Four 1-mL syringes, each containing 4 µL of the desired odor on a half square inch of filter paper, were fitted into the holes of the odor-delivering device (Galizia et al 1997).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%