2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Suggestibility, social support, and memory for a novel experience in young children

Abstract: This study examined 5- and 6-year-olds' suggestibility and interviewer demeanor as joint predictors of their memory for a novel experience. Session 1 consisted of children taking part in a novel laboratory event. Session 2 took place after approximately a 1-week delay and consisted of children completing both a memory test concerning what happened during the prior event and the Video Suggestibility Scale for Children (VSSC). During the second session, the interviewer behaved either supportively or nonsupportiv… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
42
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Conversely, the non-supportive interviewer simply fails to utilize these techniques. A majority of these studies find that a supportive interviewer increases the overall accuracy of child witness reports and decreases the amount of incorrect responses to misleading questions compared to a non-supportive interviewer (Carter, Bottoms, & Levine, 1996;Goodman, Bottoms, Schwartz-Kenney, & Rudy, 1991;Moston & Engleberg, 1992;Quas, Wallin, Papini, Lench, & Scullin, 2005).…”
Section: Social Support and Child Witnessesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conversely, the non-supportive interviewer simply fails to utilize these techniques. A majority of these studies find that a supportive interviewer increases the overall accuracy of child witness reports and decreases the amount of incorrect responses to misleading questions compared to a non-supportive interviewer (Carter, Bottoms, & Levine, 1996;Goodman, Bottoms, Schwartz-Kenney, & Rudy, 1991;Moston & Engleberg, 1992;Quas, Wallin, Papini, Lench, & Scullin, 2005).…”
Section: Social Support and Child Witnessesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2;Moston, 1992), enhances their resistance to misleading questions (Carter, Bottoms, & Levine, 1996;Davis & Bottoms, 2002;Imhoff & Baker-Ward, 1999), and reduces their suggestibility (Cornah & Memon, 1996;Greenstock & Pipe, 1996Quas, Wallin, Papini, Lench, & Scullin, 2005). Field studies to date have shown that children's cooperation can be established when they are invited to share personally meaningful information during the pre-substantive phase of the interview (Hershkowitz, 2009;Roberts, Lamb, & Sternberg, 2004;Sternberg et al, 1997) and when interviewers encourage children to talk (Teoh & Lamb, 2010).…”
Section: Benefits Of Rapport Buildingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, chronic stress, temperament, and attachment may differentially influence children's behavioral and/or physiological coping reactions (e.g., Compas, Connor-Smith, & Jaser, 2004;Goodman et al, 1997), and therefore have implications both for what children perceive as distressing and how children subsequently respond. Moreover, although there are no theoretical reasons to assume intelligence has implications for children's stress reactivity, both higher intelligence and more advanced verbal ability have been related to better memory (e.g., Quas, Wallin, Papini, Lench, & Scullin, 2005;Roebers & Schneider, 2001). Insofar as models concerning associations between stress and memory become increasingly complex, it will be important to take into account these other individual and social factors as independent or interactive contributors to children's memory.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%