1960
DOI: 10.1037/h0041915
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Supplementary report: Two-choice behavior of monkeys.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

1964
1964
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, pigeons trained in a two-key situation with response to red reinforced on a random 70% of trials, and response to yellow reinforced on the remaining trials, come to choose the red key on a random 70% of trials. Analogous experiments with the rat (Wodinsky, Candland, and Bitterman, 1958) and the monkey (Wilson, 1960;Meyer, 1960) have for the most part yielded only maximizing-i.e., choice on almost all trials of the higher-probability alternative.5 When a correspondence between reinforcement-ratio and choice-ratio has, on occasion, appeared in one of these higher animals, it has been found to have a different basis than in the lower ones. For example, a correspondence foundin the 'This work was supported by the Office of Naval Research under terms of Contract Nonr 2829(01).…”
Section: Bryn Mawr Collegementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, pigeons trained in a two-key situation with response to red reinforced on a random 70% of trials, and response to yellow reinforced on the remaining trials, come to choose the red key on a random 70% of trials. Analogous experiments with the rat (Wodinsky, Candland, and Bitterman, 1958) and the monkey (Wilson, 1960;Meyer, 1960) have for the most part yielded only maximizing-i.e., choice on almost all trials of the higher-probability alternative.5 When a correspondence between reinforcement-ratio and choice-ratio has, on occasion, appeared in one of these higher animals, it has been found to have a different basis than in the lower ones. For example, a correspondence foundin the 'This work was supported by the Office of Naval Research under terms of Contract Nonr 2829(01).…”
Section: Bryn Mawr Collegementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other experiments in which animals chose between two reinforcers of differing probabilities have produced either approximate probability matching (in which response proportions match the reinforcement probabilities) or near-exclusive preference for the alternative with the higher probability of reinforcement (e.g., Behrend & Bitterman, 1961;Bitterman, Wodinsky, & Candland, 1958;Graf, Bullock, & Bitterman, 1964;Wilson, 1960). Shimp (1966Shimp ( , 1967 proposed that pigeons' responses move toward exclusive preference if the training continues long enough.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rats apparently maximize on 70:30 spatial problems in about 100 trials (Bitterman, Wodinsky, & Candland,1958;Roberts, 1966). Rhesus monkeys (Wilson, 1960), trained on a 75:25 spatial problem with guidance, attained a level of 90 percent responses to the major stimulus only after 384 trial s. The Tange, and somewhat paradoxical distribution, of species differences in performance on spatial probability tasks suggests the possibility that qualitatively similar behavior, i.e., maximizing, may not reflect completely homologous processes in different mammalian species. …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings on cats are generally consonant with the .data for rats and monkeys. Rats (Bitterman, Wodin sky , & Candland, 1958;Roberts, 1966) and monkeys (Wilson, 1960;Wilson & Rollin, 1959) usually maximize on spatial Pl'9.bability learning tasks when tested with guidance or correction. In addition, it has been shown that monkeys learn to maximize in many fewer trials when tested with noncorrection (Wilson & Rollin,1959) than with guidance or correction (Wilson, 1960).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation