2014
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-014-0708-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Susceptible to distraction: Children lack top-down control over spatial attention capture

Abstract: Considerable evidence has indicated that adults can exert top-down control to avoid distraction by salient-but-irrelevant stimuli. However, relatively little research has explored how this ability develops across the lifespan. In the present study, we therefore assessed how well children can control the capture of spatial attention. Children (M age = 4.2 years) and adults (M age = 21.5 years) searched for target "spaceships" of a specific color while trying to ignore salient precues that either matched or mism… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0
3

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
15
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…2 In this respect, it is important to note that the relational account can accommodate many previous behavioral and electrophysiological findings that were interpreted in favor of feature-specific accounts, including feature similarity accounts and the optimal tuning account. This holds because most studies varied not only the feature of the cue (target-matching vs. nonmatching feature value) but also the relative feature of the cue (target-matching vs. nonmatching relative feature), and thus cannot distinguish between the different views (e.g., Folk & Remington, 1998;Gaspelin, Margett-Jordan, & Ruthruff, 2015;Lien et al, 2008;Lien, Ruthruff, & Johnston, 2010). For example, in search for a red target among white nontargets, observing attentional capture by a red cue but not a green cue (e.g., Folk & Remington, 1998) is consistent both with a relational account and a feature-specific account, as the red cue matches both the target feature (red) and the target's relative feature (redder), whereas the green cue matches neither the target feature (red) nor its relative feature (redder).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2 In this respect, it is important to note that the relational account can accommodate many previous behavioral and electrophysiological findings that were interpreted in favor of feature-specific accounts, including feature similarity accounts and the optimal tuning account. This holds because most studies varied not only the feature of the cue (target-matching vs. nonmatching feature value) but also the relative feature of the cue (target-matching vs. nonmatching relative feature), and thus cannot distinguish between the different views (e.g., Folk & Remington, 1998;Gaspelin, Margett-Jordan, & Ruthruff, 2015;Lien et al, 2008;Lien, Ruthruff, & Johnston, 2010). For example, in search for a red target among white nontargets, observing attentional capture by a red cue but not a green cue (e.g., Folk & Remington, 1998) is consistent both with a relational account and a feature-specific account, as the red cue matches both the target feature (red) and the target's relative feature (redder), whereas the green cue matches neither the target feature (red) nor its relative feature (redder).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…accuracies differ. Several previous studies have attempted to address this difficulty, for instance by normalizing RTs across age groups (Gaspelin, Margett-Jordan, & Ruthruff, 2015;Goldberg et al, 2001). It remains an open question, however, whether the mechanisms of selective spatial attention develop independently of basic visual sensitivity and task performance.…”
Section: Spatial Attention and Reading Abilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, children's attentional abilities and executive functions are still developing (Sarid and Breznitz, 1997; Lopez et al, 2005; Gaspelin et al, 2015) suggesting that children's visual environments may not always be advantageous (Choi et al, 2014). Indeed, while reflexive (automatic) attention is observed shortly after birth (Plude et al, 1994), other aspects of attention, such as the ability to suppress irrelevant information and sustain attention, have a rather long course of development (e.g., Ruff and Capozzoli, 2003; Rueda et al, 2004; Kannass and Colombo, 2007; Bartgis et al, 2008; Fisher et al, 2013; Gaspelin et al, 2015). It was found that toddlers are more prone to attention hijacking by distracting stimuli than are older children, and even at the age of 10 years, children are more susceptible to interference than are adults (Goldberg et al, 2001; Durston et al, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%