2018
DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1433864
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Switching between spoken language-production tasks: the role of attentional inhibition and enhancement

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Nine studies used the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), in which a deficit is typically operationalized as the relative difference in reaction time (RT) or accuracy between congruent and incongruent conditions (e.g., say “red” to the red ink color of the word red or green , respectively). The Stroop task is taken to be a measure of prepotent response inhibition, a component of inhibition that involves the ability to suppress dominant or automatic responses (Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Miyake et al, 2000a; but see Roelofs, 2021; Shao, Roelofs, Martin & Meyer, 2015; Sikora & Roelofs, 2018, for evidence against this interpretation).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nine studies used the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), in which a deficit is typically operationalized as the relative difference in reaction time (RT) or accuracy between congruent and incongruent conditions (e.g., say “red” to the red ink color of the word red or green , respectively). The Stroop task is taken to be a measure of prepotent response inhibition, a component of inhibition that involves the ability to suppress dominant or automatic responses (Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Miyake et al, 2000a; but see Roelofs, 2021; Shao, Roelofs, Martin & Meyer, 2015; Sikora & Roelofs, 2018, for evidence against this interpretation).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, although we cannot dismiss inhibition in bilingual control as we did in interpreting control (because of the language-modality connections), the concept of inhibition may not be as critical to bilingual control as hypothesized in the Inhibitory Control Model (Green, 1998) or BIA+ model (Dijkstra & Van Heuven, 2002). In section 2.3 where we argued for the view that inhibition does not capture very well the essence of language control in interpreting, many of the arguments are in fact from research on general bilingual control (Li, 1998; Yeung & Monsell, 2003; Grundy et al, 2017;Sikora & Roelofs, 2018; Dijkstra et al, 2018). “Target enhancement” (Sikora & Roelofs, 2018; or “task engagement” in Green & Abutalebi, 2013) and “task disengagement” (Green & Abutalebi, 2013; Grundy et al, 2017) have been proposed to account for empirical findings that were originally accounted for by “inhibition” (or “nontarget inhibition”).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In section 2.3 where we argued for the view that inhibition does not capture very well the essence of language control in interpreting, many of the arguments are in fact from research on general bilingual control (Li, 1998;Yeung & Monsell, 2003;Grundy et al, 2017;Sikora & Roelofs, 2018Dijkstra et al, 2018). "Target enhancement" (Sikora & Roelofs, 2018; or "task engagement" in Green & Abutalebi, 2013) and "task disengagement" (Green & Abutalebi, 2013;Grundy et al, 2017) have been proposed to account for empirical findings that were originally accounted for by "inhibition" (or "nontarget inhibition"). Specifically, in a language-switching naming task, to achieve adequate processing in the weaker L2, one has to focus on it and enhance its activation, which would then require more processing time to get disengaged from it, leading to more time to switch to L1 than the other way around.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A multitude of studies have shown that control processes, measured with the switching task, are implemented during both single (e.g., Finkbeiner, Almeida, Janssen & Caramazza, 2006; Sikora & Roelofs, 2018; Yeung & Monsell, 2003) and dual (e.g., Bonfieni, Branigan, Pickering & Sorace, 2019; Costa, Santesteban & Ivanova, 2006; Meuter & Allport, 1999) language production. However, few studies have directly compared control processes in a single and dual language context (Abutalebi, Annoni, Zimine, Pegna, Seghier, Lee-Jahnke, Lazeyras, Cappa & Khateb, 2008; Declerck et al, 2017b; for comparisons of different types of bilingual language control, see also Dias, Villameriel, Giezen, Costello & Carreiras, 2017; Kirk, Kempe, Scott-Brown, Philipp & Declerck, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%