2003
DOI: 10.3758/bf03196483
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Syllogistic reasoning time: Disconfirmation disconfirmed

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
73
0
7

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
4
73
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite the differences in the nature of the response conflicts that arose in the present paradigm compared to the traditional belief bias paradigm, we have demonstrated some important parallels with findings arising in the more established literature on belief bias. One key correspondence concerns the observation that conflict problems in both paradigms show inflated response times relative to non-conflict problems (Ball et al, 2006;Stupple & Ball, 2008;Thompson et al, 2003), which is fully in line with dual-process predictions that derive from either a parallel process perspective (e.g., Sloman, 2002;Stupple & Ball, 2008) or a default-interventionist perspective such as the selective processing model espoused by Evans (e.g., 2000; see also ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 75%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Despite the differences in the nature of the response conflicts that arose in the present paradigm compared to the traditional belief bias paradigm, we have demonstrated some important parallels with findings arising in the more established literature on belief bias. One key correspondence concerns the observation that conflict problems in both paradigms show inflated response times relative to non-conflict problems (Ball et al, 2006;Stupple & Ball, 2008;Thompson et al, 2003), which is fully in line with dual-process predictions that derive from either a parallel process perspective (e.g., Sloman, 2002;Stupple & Ball, 2008) or a default-interventionist perspective such as the selective processing model espoused by Evans (e.g., 2000; see also ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…This response time pattern shows similarity to that displayed in the belief bias paradigm, in which the problems exhibiting the most inflated response times are invalid-believable conflict items, where conclusion believability must be inhibited while analytic processing occurs (see Thompson et al, 2003, for the first reported observation of this effect, and Stupple & Ball, 2008, for a replication). It could be argued that increased response times for the invalid matching conflict problems in the present experiment likewise arose from the effort made to inhibit a heuristically-based matching response while analytic processes were applied in an attempt to determine the logical status of the conclusion.…”
Section: Response Timesmentioning
confidence: 69%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These problems are difficult and demanding, and are therefore appropriate for examining the potential problems of fatigue and expectancy. For example, a recent study using identical materials (Thompson, Striemer, Reikoff, Gunter, & Campbell, 2003) indicated that participants made errors on 35% of the problems (i.e., by accepting invalid or rejecting valid conclusions), and required an average of over 20 sec per problem to solve them.…”
Section: Rationale For the Current Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, there is surprisingly little direct chronometric evidence for this claim. As has reasoning and memory research in general, dual-process-related studies have been mainly concerned with response accuracy data and not with response latencies (Kahana & Loftus, 1999;Thompson, Striemer, Reikoff, Gunter, & Campbell, 2003). Despite over 25 years of experimenting with the conjunction fallacy a basic latency analysis (i.e., comparing inference times for correct vs. heuristic responses) has not yet been presented.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%