2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-016-1203-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tactile suppression in goal-directed movement

Abstract: Sharing numerous characteristics with suppression in the other senses, tactile suppression is a reliable phenomenon that accompanies movement. By investigating the simplest of movements (e.g., finger flexions), early research tried to explain the origins of the phenomenon in terms of motor command generation together with sensory reafference. Here, we review recent research that has delved into (naturalistic) goal-directed movements. In connection with goal-directed movement, tactile suppression is evident as … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
36
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 142 publications
(192 reference statements)
7
36
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Given the interactive possibilities of altering perceived surface texture via sound [for use in VR shopping applications, Ho et al (2013) for example], the bi-directional relationship between sensory perception and movement is relevant to consider. Recent work shows that changes in sensory perception during movement are specific to the type of sensory input and its relevance to the movement concerned (Juravle and Spence, 2011;Juravle et al, 2017). For example, when auditory cues are paired with a juggling task, there is an increased sensitivity to detect a gap in the auditory cue when compared with a rest (no movement) condition, but the opposite occurs for tactile cues: reduced sensitivity occurs during the movement (juggling) condition than during rest (see Figure 5) (Juravle and Spence, 2011).…”
Section: Interim Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given the interactive possibilities of altering perceived surface texture via sound [for use in VR shopping applications, Ho et al (2013) for example], the bi-directional relationship between sensory perception and movement is relevant to consider. Recent work shows that changes in sensory perception during movement are specific to the type of sensory input and its relevance to the movement concerned (Juravle and Spence, 2011;Juravle et al, 2017). For example, when auditory cues are paired with a juggling task, there is an increased sensitivity to detect a gap in the auditory cue when compared with a rest (no movement) condition, but the opposite occurs for tactile cues: reduced sensitivity occurs during the movement (juggling) condition than during rest (see Figure 5) (Juravle and Spence, 2011).…”
Section: Interim Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tactile suppression, in particular, refers to the attenuation of tactile signals during movement planning and execution when presented on a moving limb compared to rest (for review, see Juravle, Binsted, & Spence, 2017). Originally, tactile suppression was primarily considered as a cancellation of specific afferences that are predicted based on the efference copy of the motor commands (e.g., Bays, Flanagan, & Wolpert, 2006; but see Chapman & Beauchamp, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The activation of infragranular layers V and VI of M1 was followed by a period of tactile suppression, with an amplitude and time course similar to that observed following intracortical microstimulation of M1 in monkeys 25 . Such tactile suppression is also observed during movement [26][27][28][29] , and is thought to inhibit responses to self-generated tactile stimulation. Surprisingly, previous studies examining the effects of M1 optogenetic stimulation on whisker-evoked responses in mice have found predominantly facilitatory effects 8,9 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Tactile suppression is likely to be mediated at multiple levels within sensorimotor systems, and may be effected by corollary motor commands and proprioceptive reafference 3,8,10,[26][27][28][29][30] . Here, we find that activation of M1 NTSR1+ layer VI cortico-thalamic neurons induces an excitationsuppression sequence across thalamocotical circuits, suggesting that cortico-thalamic projections from M1 to somatosensory thalamus are sufficient to induce some aspects of tactile suppression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%