2018
DOI: 10.3758/s13420-018-0314-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Taking an insect-inspired approach to bird navigation

Abstract: Navigation is an essential skill for many animals, and understanding how animal use environmental information, particularly visual information, to navigate has a long history in both ethology and psychology. In birds, the dominant approach for investigating navigation at small-scales comes from comparative psychology, which emphasizes the cognitive representations underpinning spatial memory. The majority of this work is based in the laboratory and it is unclear whether this context itself affects the informat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 125 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…By analyzing the navigation of birds and insects (Pritchard and Healy 2018), one can also test that they specify a referent direction that in the presence of deflecting winds does not coincide with the actual flight direction. To test this suggestion, one can attach an insect to a motionless surface while blowing the wind to evoke rhythmical motion of the wings.…”
Section: Testing and Advancing The Referent Control Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By analyzing the navigation of birds and insects (Pritchard and Healy 2018), one can also test that they specify a referent direction that in the presence of deflecting winds does not coincide with the actual flight direction. To test this suggestion, one can attach an insect to a motionless surface while blowing the wind to evoke rhythmical motion of the wings.…”
Section: Testing and Advancing The Referent Control Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because 7 out 10 birds in the second phase of Experiment 3, when we lowered the concentration of the second flower, kept the origin of their trapline but reversed the direction in which they flew around the flowers, it seems plausible that these birds did just invert the direction. We do know, however, that wild hummingbirds learn the locations of flowers in relation to visual landmark and that movement vectors are thought to be used over long distances during migration (Pritchard & Healy, 2018). What information they use when moving between multiple locations is yet to be determined, but comparing the traplining behaviour of different taxa might be able to tell us something more about the type of information animals use when solving these multi-location problems.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Vandersteen et al stated that representative light measurements should include the horizon because it contains light stimuli in the direction of movement (Vandersteen et al, 2020 ). Given that air‐borne animals are not restricted to the nadir or horizontal view for orientation (El Jundi et al, 2015 ; Foster et al, 2018 ; Pritchard & Healy, 2018 ), high‐resolution full‐sphere photometric measurements are desirable (see Figure 2 ; Jechow et al, 2019 ; Vandersteen et al, 2020 ).…”
Section: From Surface To Volumementioning
confidence: 99%