2014
DOI: 10.1037/cns0000021
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tales of the unexpected: Attentional awareness, goal relevance, and prior exposure to an unexpected change.

Abstract: We report an experiment where we examine whether an unexpected stimulus change that occurs whilst performing an engaging task is more likely to be noticed if it is relevant or irrelevant to the goal of that task. The goal was to count the number of times moving targets (white letters) hit the side of the frame on a computer screen but to ignore similarly moving distractors (black letters). We found that a highly goal-relevant change (i.e., a black distractor changing into a white target) was more likely to be … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In a standard IB task participants are required to track a series of white Ls and Ts (but ignore similarly moving black Ls and Ts) as they move randomly on the screen when, after a few seconds, a moving red cross appears for several seconds (based on Most et al, 2001; see also Simons, 2003). However, the status of this unexpected stimulus is ambiguous since it is not clear whether the most appropriate strategy in this task would be to, for example, ignore it, process it or process it and then, at a later stage inhibit it (Richards, Hannon, Vohra, & Golan, 2014;Richards, Hannon, & Derakshan, 2010;Richards, Hannon, & Vitkovitch, 2010). In order to manipulate the status of the unexpected stimulus to make it relevant during the dynamic task we used a new task in which one of the target stimuli undergoes an unexpected change directly relevant to the goal (one target, i.e., a green T, turns suddenly into another type of target, i.e., a blue F).…”
Section: Experimental Paradigm and Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In a standard IB task participants are required to track a series of white Ls and Ts (but ignore similarly moving black Ls and Ts) as they move randomly on the screen when, after a few seconds, a moving red cross appears for several seconds (based on Most et al, 2001; see also Simons, 2003). However, the status of this unexpected stimulus is ambiguous since it is not clear whether the most appropriate strategy in this task would be to, for example, ignore it, process it or process it and then, at a later stage inhibit it (Richards, Hannon, Vohra, & Golan, 2014;Richards, Hannon, & Derakshan, 2010;Richards, Hannon, & Vitkovitch, 2010). In order to manipulate the status of the unexpected stimulus to make it relevant during the dynamic task we used a new task in which one of the target stimuli undergoes an unexpected change directly relevant to the goal (one target, i.e., a green T, turns suddenly into another type of target, i.e., a blue F).…”
Section: Experimental Paradigm and Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This failure may be due to the power of intrinsic oscillatory activity that prevents visual information from low-level processor to be granted access to awareness (mainly in theta, alpha and beta band: see Dehaene & Changeux, 2011;Jensen, Bonnefond, & VanRullen, 2012, Klimesch, 1999. Another possibility is that the unexpected stimulus/change may be processed but then inhibited and prevented from accessing awareness due to it being irrelevant to the primary task (Richards, Hannon, Vohra, & Golan, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The IB task was programmed in MatLab™ and based on the work of Richards, Hannon, Vohra, and Golan (2014), where the unexpected event does not involve any addition of new stimuli, but rather an unexpected change to one of the stimuli already present in the visual dynamic scene (Fig. 1A).…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some researchers argue that failing to notice an unexpected stimulus occurs when early mechanism of exogenous attention and visual working memory (VWM) are predominantly involved in another task, resulting in too few resources remaining for the processing of an unexpected stimulus or change (Papera & Richards, 2016). Other accounts propose that in most IB tasks the unexpected event is not relevant to the primary task, making it susceptible to inhibition and therefore prevented from reaching awareness (Richards, Hannon, Vohra, & Golan, 2014). Furthermore, neural network modelling has argued that intrinsic oscillatory brain activity in high-order brain areas (particularly in the alpha and theta band: Mathewson, Gratton, Fabiani, Beck, & Ro, 2009;Jensen, Bonnefond, & VanRullen, 2012;Papera & Richards, 2016) may prevent low-level processors (i.e., parietal areas) to "ignite" a widespread activation across several regions that are thought to reflect the current conscious content (Dehaene & Changeux, 2005Dehaene, Sergen, & Changeux, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%