2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1126-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Target–object integration, attention distribution, and object orientation interactively modulate object-based selection

Abstract: The representational basis of attentional selection can be object-based. Various studies have suggested,

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

18
49
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
18
49
0
Order By: Relevance
“…reported (Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016). In summary, these findings demonstrate that the SDA is (1) driven by target locations that require a shift of attention across the visual field meridians,…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 51%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…reported (Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016). In summary, these findings demonstrate that the SDA is (1) driven by target locations that require a shift of attention across the visual field meridians,…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 51%
“…is frequently reported for horizontal objects, whereas a same object cost (i.e., slower RTs to invalid locations on a cued object versus a non-cued object) has been reported for vertical objects (Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016;Conci & Müller, 2009;Harrison & Feldman, 2009;Hein, Blaschke, & Rolke, 2016;Pilz, Roggeveen, Creighton, Bennet, & Sekular, 2012). Our work previously investigated this dissociation by comparing the reallocation of object-based attention across the horizontal versus vertical meridian using an 'L'-shaped object (Barnas & Greenberg, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Example of a trial with the cue in the upper-left position, dissimilar rectangle color, and L as the target (F are distractors). Please note that the horizontal meridian score (left displays) takes into account only vertical shifts of attention and thus controls for a potential facilitation effect along the horizontal meridian, as suggested by the meridian facilitation theory (e.g., Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016;Pilz et al, 2012). The vertical meridian score (right displays), on the other hand, takes into account only horizontal shifts of attention.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3), F(4, 84) = 2.56, p = 0.04. We found, here, a cuevalidity effect for all object types (55 ms for visible objects, 102 ms for occluded objects and 88 ms for illusory objects; all ps < 0.01), but a same-object advantage of 29 ms emerged only for the visible objects, t(21) = 2.23, p = 0.04 (for inconsistent findings see Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016). All other t-tests were not significant (all ps > 0.05, and all ts < 1.90).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 57%