2007
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-0946-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Task-switching in schizophrenia: active switching costs and passive carry-over effects in an antisaccade paradigm

Abstract: It has been hypothesized that impaired task-switching underlies some of the behavioural deficits in schizophrenia. However, task-switching involves many cognitive operations. In this study our goal was to isolate the effects on latency and accuracy that can be attributed to specific task-switch processes, by studying the inter-trial effects in blocks of randomly mixed prosaccades and antisaccades. By varying the preparatory interval between an instructional cue and the target, we assessed the costs of both (1)… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our findings show no task-switch effects during the long-lead condition, but anti-saccade responses in the simultaneous condition showed a task-switch cost. This same effect was also observed by Barton et al (2006), Greenzang et al (2007), and Weber (1995), who noted a task-switch cost at short cue to target intervals (200 ms). Studies reporting task-switch benefits for anti-saccades (Barton et al, 2002, 2006; Cherkasova et al, 2002; Manoach et al, 2002) used a considerably longer cue-to- target interval (two sec) than we used here.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Our findings show no task-switch effects during the long-lead condition, but anti-saccade responses in the simultaneous condition showed a task-switch cost. This same effect was also observed by Barton et al (2006), Greenzang et al (2007), and Weber (1995), who noted a task-switch cost at short cue to target intervals (200 ms). Studies reporting task-switch benefits for anti-saccades (Barton et al, 2002, 2006; Cherkasova et al, 2002; Manoach et al, 2002) used a considerably longer cue-to- target interval (two sec) than we used here.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This task was administered on a different day from the Demand Selection Task. This condition was administered as a type of control as we anticipated that subjects who experienced greater switching costs might be more effort averse, and further expected that patients, as a group, might show increased switching costs (although the evidence in the literature on this point is conflicting; see Greenzang, Manoach, Goff, Barton, 2007; Ravizza, Moua, Long, Carter, 2010; Wylie, Clark, Butler, Javitt, 2010). …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a result of impaired dorsolateral PFC activity prior to antisaccades, relatives may rely more heavily upon orbital PFC than control subjects in an attempt to inhibit prepotent reflexive saccades. Recent work has provided evidence that a failure of schizophrenia patients to inhibit reflexive saccades (Manoach, Lindgren, Cherkasova, Goff, Halpern, et al, 2002) and not deficient switching between tasks (Greenzang, Manoach, Goff, & Barton, 2007) leads to poor antisaccade performance. We found schizophrenia patients had larger frontal polar potentials than controls from 120 to 0 ms prior to step prosaccade potentials.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%