Brain Mapping 2015
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-397025-1.00250-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Task Switching Processes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
63
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 84 publications
(119 reference statements)
4
63
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Mirelman et al (2011), for instance, reported better TMT performance in Parkinson's disease patients after dual task treadmill training. Furthermore, task switching requires cognitive control and has been found in many studies to rely on the cognitive control network (e.g., Jamadar, Thienel, & Karayanidis, 2015) which comprises SPL activation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mirelman et al (2011), for instance, reported better TMT performance in Parkinson's disease patients after dual task treadmill training. Furthermore, task switching requires cognitive control and has been found in many studies to rely on the cognitive control network (e.g., Jamadar, Thienel, & Karayanidis, 2015) which comprises SPL activation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These paradigms (Allport et al, ; Meiran, Chorev, & Sapir, ; Monsell, ; Rogers & Monsell, ; Rubinstein, Meyer, & Evan, ) target cognitive control mechanisms required to shift between goals or task sets (e.g., classifying the number 3 based on parity [odd vs. even] or magnitude [< 5 or > 5], see Figure d; for overview of paradigms, see Kiesel et al, , and Jamadar, Thienel, & Karayanidis, ; see also Baniqued et al, ; Provost et al, ). Here, cognitive control processes are required to implement the information provided by external cues (e.g., the letters P or M, to indicate parity or magnitude, presented prior to the stimulus, see Figure d) or internal cues (e.g., implement an instruction to change task every two trials) on the current target.…”
Section: Tests and Paradigms Used To Assess Or Manipulate Cognitive Cmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By and large, DAN is activated during conditions in which control is needed to select, maintain, or switch between one or more sets of stimulus‐response rules that are mapped to different contextually defined task goals. Functional MRI studies have shown that components of this network are activated in task‐switching paradigms, and more strongly so for switch than for repeat trials (for reviews, see De Baene, Kuhn, & Brass, ; Ruge, Jamadar, Zimmermann, & Karayanidis, ; see activation likelihood estimation [ALE] meta‐analyses in Jamadar et al, ). This network could therefore be viewed as supporting “shifting” operations in Miyake and Friedman's view.…”
Section: Brain Mechanisms In Support Of Cognitive Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The ability to flexibly adapt behavior to meet changing contextual demands is the hallmark of cognitive control. A large body of research has used variations of the task‐switching paradigm to study cognitive control processes (for recent reviews, see Jamadar, Thienel, & Karayanidis, ; Vandierendonck, Liefooghe, & Verbruggen, ) and has shown that both proactive and reactive control processes play a role in effective cognitive flexibility (Braver, Gray, & Burgess, ). In task‐switching paradigms, task switch trials are associated with poorer performance than task repeat trials.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%