2018
DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000571
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal constraints in the use of auditory action effects for motor optimization.

Abstract: For quick ballistic movements the possibility of making online adjustments is limited. However, when the same action (e.g., pressing a button) is repeated multiple times, trial-by-trial adjustments are possible: Previous studies found that participants utilized auditory effects as feedback to optimize the applied force for such tone eliciting actions. In the current study, it was examined whether this also occurred if a delay was inserted between the action and its auditory effect. In 2 experiments, participan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
(106 reference statements)
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In both experiments, actions following tone-eliciting actions were softer than those following silent actions. Because the arrival of the tone effect was unpredictable, the results presented here—along with previous results (Neszmélyi & Horváth, 2018, 2019)—support the notion that action-effect–related motor adaptation is—at least in part—based on automatic action-effect integration that does not take the wider context of the actions into consideration. That is, tones are used to optimize the force of the actions, even if this strategy could be associated with additional switching costs and the unpredictability of the auditory effect for subsequent movements could limit anticipation-based processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…In both experiments, actions following tone-eliciting actions were softer than those following silent actions. Because the arrival of the tone effect was unpredictable, the results presented here—along with previous results (Neszmélyi & Horváth, 2018, 2019)—support the notion that action-effect–related motor adaptation is—at least in part—based on automatic action-effect integration that does not take the wider context of the actions into consideration. That is, tones are used to optimize the force of the actions, even if this strategy could be associated with additional switching costs and the unpredictability of the auditory effect for subsequent movements could limit anticipation-based processes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…Second, when tone-eliciting and silent actions are intermixed, utilization of tones for finding the optimal force might require switching between different control strategies. As indicated by previous studies (Horváth et al, 2018; Neszmélyi & Horváth, 2018, 2019), the balance between the goal to apply the least amount of force and the goal to perform the action successfully is at different force levels for tone-eliciting and silent actions. For tone-eliciting actions, tones provide reliable feedback about successful action execution, whereas for silent actions, uncertainty about action success could result in increased force application.…”
mentioning
confidence: 73%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The first possibility may find support from the observation that distal sensory feedback (e.g., a sound) can reduce keypress intensity (Chase et al 1961 ; Neszmelyi and Horvath 2018 ). Also in study 1 (no keypress force manipulation), keypresses in the AS condition (mean = 1.44 Newton; SD = 0.55) had lighter peak force than in the AO condition [mean = 1.72 Newton; SD = 0.94; t (41) = − 3.32, p = 9.38e−4, one-tailed, d z = − 0.51].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%