1996
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1996.66-311
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal Control by Progressive‐interval Schedules of Reinforcement

Abstract: Progressive-interval performances are described using measures that have proven to be successful in the analysis of fixed-interval responding. Five rats were trained with schedules in which the durations of consecutive intervals increased arithmetically as each interval was completed (either 6-s or 12-s steps for different subjects). The response patterns that emerged with extended training (90 sessions) indicated that performances had come under temporal control. Postreinforcement pausing increased as a funct… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0
2

Year Published

1998
1998
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
1
5
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The data intersect the y -axis at 2 s, and increase thereafter with a slope of m = 0.51, indicating that these pigeons tended to pause for half the length of the prior IRI. Such linear pausing is consistent with research on progressive interval, fixed ratio, cyclic ratio, fixed interval, cyclic interval and response-initiated delay schedules (Bizo & Killeen, 1997; Capehart, Eckerman, Guilkey, & Shull, 1980; Hanson & Killeen, 1981; Leinenweber, Nietzel, & Baron, 1996; Leslie, Boyle, & Shaw, 2000; Schneider, 1969; Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996). The reason for this invariance is uncertain, but the simple model of Capehart and associates has some face validity.…”
Section: Experiments 3: a Basis-6 Pr Schedulesupporting
confidence: 86%
“…The data intersect the y -axis at 2 s, and increase thereafter with a slope of m = 0.51, indicating that these pigeons tended to pause for half the length of the prior IRI. Such linear pausing is consistent with research on progressive interval, fixed ratio, cyclic ratio, fixed interval, cyclic interval and response-initiated delay schedules (Bizo & Killeen, 1997; Capehart, Eckerman, Guilkey, & Shull, 1980; Hanson & Killeen, 1981; Leinenweber, Nietzel, & Baron, 1996; Leslie, Boyle, & Shaw, 2000; Schneider, 1969; Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996). The reason for this invariance is uncertain, but the simple model of Capehart and associates has some face validity.…”
Section: Experiments 3: a Basis-6 Pr Schedulesupporting
confidence: 86%
“…By contrast, in true PI schedules, each IRI is mathematically related to the previous one. Despite the differences between the present yoked-interval progressions and PI progressions defined by either arithmetic or geometric progressions (e.g., Dougherty, Cherek, & Roache, 1994;Harzem, 1969;Leinenweber, Nietzel, & Baron, 1996), the present behavioral effects of the yoked-interval schedule were similar in that response rates decreased and PRPs increased as the sessions, and thus the IRIs, progressed. Comparisons of the cumulative records in the present Figures 2 and 5 to those presented by Leinenweber et al, however, indicate that PI schedule response patterns were more positively accelerated and the response patterns under the present yoked-interval schedules were more linear.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…Most studies of the effect of interval schedules of reinforcement have used a constant expected value (Catania & Reynolds, 1968) in which tracking could not be observed, but procedures in which the expected value changes in an orderly way have produced temporal tracking. This has been shown in progressive-interval schedules, in which each interval from food to the next available food in a session is longer than the previous one (Harzem, 1969;Leinenweber, Nietzel, & Baron, 1996), cyclical-interval schedules, in which each interval from food to the next available food gradually increases and then decreases throughout a session (Innis, 1981;Innis & Staddon, 1970, 1971Staddon, 1964;, and cyclical, response-initiated delay schedules, in which the interval from the first response after food until the next food gradually increases and then decreases throughout a session (Higa, Thaw, & Staddon, 1993;Innis, Cooper, & Mitchell, 1993;Innis, Mitchell, & Staddon, 1993;Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996). Tracking in cyclical schedules has been found when intervals are changed by various functions, such as arithmetic, logarithmic, and geometric (Innis & Staddon, 1971).…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%