2021
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251480
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal discounting when outcomes are experienced in the moment: Validation of a novel paradigm and comparison with a classic hypothetical intertemporal choice task

Abstract: When faced with intertemporal choices, people typically devalue rewards available in the future compared to rewards more immediately available, a phenomenon known as temporal discounting. Decisions involving intertemporal choices arise daily, with critical impact on health and financial wellbeing. Although many such decisions are “experiential” in that they involve delays and rewards that are experienced in real-time and can inform subsequent choices, most studies have focused on intertemporal choices with hyp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 127 publications
(194 reference statements)
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, even when examining baseline discounting in association with trait (or even state) anxiety or depression, we failed to observe significant associations, whereas such associations do, at times, emerge in clinical samples. For example, in a study by Steinglass et al, 2017, although there was no evidence of an association between trait anxiety and discounting in the control group (also see e.g., Patt et al, 2021;Jenks & Lawyer, 2015), such associations did emerge in some of their clinical samples (higher trait anxiety in individuals with Obsessive…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Indeed, even when examining baseline discounting in association with trait (or even state) anxiety or depression, we failed to observe significant associations, whereas such associations do, at times, emerge in clinical samples. For example, in a study by Steinglass et al, 2017, although there was no evidence of an association between trait anxiety and discounting in the control group (also see e.g., Patt et al, 2021;Jenks & Lawyer, 2015), such associations did emerge in some of their clinical samples (higher trait anxiety in individuals with Obsessive…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, even when examining baseline discounting in association with trait (or even state) anxiety or depression, we failed to observe significant associations, whereas such associations do, at times, emerge in clinical samples. For example, in a study by Steinglass et al, 2017, although there was no evidence of an association between trait anxiety and discounting in the control group (also see Jenks & Lawyer, 2015;Patt et al, 2021), such associations did emerge in some of their clinical samples (higher trait anxiety in individuals with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder and Social Anxiety Disorder was associated with discounting less steeply). Although we did observe a large range of scores on all clinical measures in our sample, it is possible that a relationship between discounting and aspects of anxiety or depression only manifests at a higher level of symptoms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For example, although we controlled English ability for all three studies, it was difficult to balance familiarity between Chinese and English. While measuring intertemporal choice, a less hypothetical paradigm (Thoma & Tytus, 2018), e.g., with real waiting time or delayed payment (Patt, Hunsberger, Jones, Keane, & Verfaellie, 2021;Xu, González-Vallejo, & Vincent, 2020), may lead participants to become more emotionally involved in the intertemporal context, thus providing a more sensitive and ecologically valid measurement.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%