APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis, Vol. 1: Methods and Principles. 2013
DOI: 10.1037/13937-014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The allocation of operant behavior.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 180 publications
(240 reference statements)
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…According to the same variable‐interval schedules, a choice of left or right led to one of two outcomes with different work requirements for reward. The primary advantage of the concurrent‐chains procedure is that conditions in the choice phase are otherwise equal for the two alternatives, and that preference for one or the other alternative can only be determined by differences between alternatives in the outcome phase (Grace & Hucks, ), or in the present case, by the biasing effect of a prior investment. In the choice phase of Magalhães and White's () procedure, two components signaled by green and red cues were in effect.…”
Section: The Sunk Cost Effect In Nonhuman Animals: Laboratory Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to the same variable‐interval schedules, a choice of left or right led to one of two outcomes with different work requirements for reward. The primary advantage of the concurrent‐chains procedure is that conditions in the choice phase are otherwise equal for the two alternatives, and that preference for one or the other alternative can only be determined by differences between alternatives in the outcome phase (Grace & Hucks, ), or in the present case, by the biasing effect of a prior investment. In the choice phase of Magalhães and White's () procedure, two components signaled by green and red cues were in effect.…”
Section: The Sunk Cost Effect In Nonhuman Animals: Laboratory Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Initial‐link responding provides access to one of two mutually exclusive terminal links. The ratio or proportion of responses in the initial link provides a direct behavioral measure of preference for terminal‐link outcomes (Fantino, ; Grace & Hucks, ; Herrnstein, ). Two technical features of the present concurrent‐chains procedure are important.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This Generalized Matching Law (GML) has been shown to accurately describe a wide range of choice data at the individual‐subject level in a number of species (see review by Grace & Hucks, ). The IFD can be similarly expressed in generalized form (Bell & Baum, ; Fagen, ; Kennedy & Gray, ): logN1N2=s.logR1R2+log b, where N 1 and N 2 are the number of foragers observed at Patch 1 and 2, respectively, R 1 and R 2 are the total reinforcers (or resources) delivered at Patch 1 and Patch 2, s has been conceptualized as either the sensitivity of the group to the resource distribution (Kennedy & Gray, ) or the inverse of the level of interference (Sutherland, ; Fagen, ), and b is the site bias (Bell & Baum, ; Kennedy & Gray, ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The sensitivity of behavior to reinforcement is represented by s (the slope of the resulting linear function) and b represents bias towards one patch or another (corresponds to the y-intercept of the function). This Generalized Matching Law (GML) has been shown to accurately describe a wide range of choice data at the individual-subject level in a number of species (see review by Grace & Hucks, 2013). The IFD can be similarly expressed in generalized form (Bell & Baum, 2002;Fagen, 1987;Kennedy & Gray, 1993):…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%