2001
DOI: 10.1007/bf03173023
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The assessment of quantitative problem-solving skills with “none of the above”-items (NOTA items)

Abstract: In this contribution we concentrate on the features of a particular item format: items having as the last option "none of the above" (NOTA items). There is considerable dispute on the advisability of the usage of NOTA items in testing. Some authors come to the conclusion that NOTA items should be avoided, some come to neutral conclusions while others argue that NOTA items are optimal test items. In this article, we provide evidence to this discussion by conducting protocol analysis on written statements of exa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Kubinger and Wolfsbauer (2010: 303) contend that from the point of view of personality psychology, examinees in a multiple-choice test are likely to differ in the way they deal with multiple choice items, which might impact their results. Dochy et al (2001) on their side presume that, if an examinee reaches a particular answer for an item in a multiple-choice test, which, however, is not offered as an option, then, the higher the examinee’s general assertiveness is, the more he/she uses “none of the other options is right.” Ávila and Torrubia (2004) established that extraverted, impulsive, and low-anxiety examinees give more incorrect responses in multiple-choice tasks but make fewer omission errors and Alker et al (1969) indicate that examinees with a “nonconformist” personality score higher on achievement tests than others because of fewer skipped items and not at all using the “I don’t know the solution” option. Lastly, Stoeber and Kersting (2007) showed that “perfectionist” examinees attain higher scores in achievement tests.…”
Section: A Process-performance Approach To Intelligence Test Results:...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kubinger and Wolfsbauer (2010: 303) contend that from the point of view of personality psychology, examinees in a multiple-choice test are likely to differ in the way they deal with multiple choice items, which might impact their results. Dochy et al (2001) on their side presume that, if an examinee reaches a particular answer for an item in a multiple-choice test, which, however, is not offered as an option, then, the higher the examinee’s general assertiveness is, the more he/she uses “none of the other options is right.” Ávila and Torrubia (2004) established that extraverted, impulsive, and low-anxiety examinees give more incorrect responses in multiple-choice tasks but make fewer omission errors and Alker et al (1969) indicate that examinees with a “nonconformist” personality score higher on achievement tests than others because of fewer skipped items and not at all using the “I don’t know the solution” option. Lastly, Stoeber and Kersting (2007) showed that “perfectionist” examinees attain higher scores in achievement tests.…”
Section: A Process-performance Approach To Intelligence Test Results:...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As such, they can adversely affect the technical quality of the item because students who may know very little about the content of interest may be able to answer the question correctly. In addition, questions, especially complex questions, with "none of the above" as an answer, may disproportionately attract student responses because of minor misconceptions or procedural errors on the part of the student (Docy, Moerkerke, DeCorte, & Segers, 2001). • Choose distractors in order to pinpoint common misconceptions and provide educators information, which will be useful in follow-on instruction.…”
Section: Test Development Strategymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In order to maintain the independence mentioned above, caution should be exercised in using the options “All of the above” and “None of the above.” Nevertheless, if it is decided to use them, it is important to bear in mind the following: The former appears to introduce an additional difficulty (Dudycha & Carpenter, 1973; Mueller, 1975), especially for subjects with low levels of knowledge (Martínez, Moreno, Martín, Trigo, & López, 2004), probably because it requires them to know that at least two of the above are correct. For its part, the option “None of the above” has a general difficulty effect (Dochy, Moerkerke, De Corte, & Segers, 2001;Haladyna et al, 2002), at least when it is constructed as the correct option (Martínez et al, 2004), probably because it involves negative language and logic, referring to what things are not, an indirect and normally more complicated form than referring to them in positive terms.…”
Section: Proposal For New Guidelinesmentioning
confidence: 99%