2002
DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01947-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

18
239
2
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 331 publications
(261 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
18
239
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, the activation described falls in a (medial) region of the inferior frontal gyrus, very different from the (lateral) sections typically implicated in syntactic tasks (e.g. Bornkessel et al, 2005;Dapretto andBookheimer, 1999, c.f., Grodzinsky andKaan and Swaab, 2002;Price, 2000). The medial region reported by Prado et al (2010a), often referred to as the "deep frontal operculum" (Anwander et al, 2007), has been proposed to serve articulatory planning (Price, 2000).…”
Section: Deduction As a Language Based Processmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, the activation described falls in a (medial) region of the inferior frontal gyrus, very different from the (lateral) sections typically implicated in syntactic tasks (e.g. Bornkessel et al, 2005;Dapretto andBookheimer, 1999, c.f., Grodzinsky andKaan and Swaab, 2002;Price, 2000). The medial region reported by Prado et al (2010a), often referred to as the "deep frontal operculum" (Anwander et al, 2007), has been proposed to serve articulatory planning (Price, 2000).…”
Section: Deduction As a Language Based Processmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, we were interested in the nature of the cortical systems that compute syntactic representations during online language comprehension. Three regions have traditionally been identified as candidates for syntactic processing (Fedorenko et al, 2012;Friederici and Kotz, 2003;Grodzinsky and Friederici, 2006;Kaan and Swaab, 2002). Historically, Broca's area or left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) has been most associated with syntax (Ben-Shachar et al, 2003;Ben-Shachar et al, 2004;Bornkessel-Schlesewsky et al, 2009;Caplan et al, 2008;Caplan et al, 2000;Carramazza and Zurif, 1976;Dapretto and Bookheimer, 1999;Embick et al, 2000;Friederici et al, 2006;Grodzinsky, 2001;Just et al, 1996;Santi and Grodzinsky, 2007;Stromswold et al, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, although the different ERP effects are indicative of the engagement of at least partially non-overlapping neural systems, they do not tell us which exact cortical networks are involved in the establishing of reference, or what their relationship is to the networks known to be involved in the processing of meaning. In the last decade, language comprehension researchers have begun to use functional neuroimaging techniques (e.g., fMRI) to unravel the neuronal infrastructure of semantic, syntactic and phonological processing (e.g., Bookheimer, 2002;Friederici, 2002;Kaan and Swaab, 2002;Stowe et al, 2005, for reviews). However, with the exception of a few studies focusing on cohesion/coherence processing in discourse comprehension (Ferstl and von Cramon, 2001;Robertson et al, 2000), there has been no functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) work on the neural substrate for referential processing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, referential ambiguity may require the language system to, from the ambiguous pronoun and onwards, actively maintain two candidate fillers for an unresolved single referential slot in working memory (e.g., Gibson, 1998). In such a scenario, the brain signature of referential ambiguity may resemble that of increased working memory load, eliciting increased activity in the LIFG (BA 44/45/47;Fletcher and Henson, 2001;Kaan and Swaab, 2002;Smith and Jonides, 1999). Second, in a recent review, Novick et al (2005) posit a similar prediction for LIFG involvement in resolving referential ambiguity, albeit for different reasons.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%