1998
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9507.00062
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Cognitive Basis of Future‐oriented Prosocial Behavior

Abstract: Two experiments examined the development of future-oriented prosocial

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
97
1
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 151 publications
(107 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
8
97
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We know that 2-and 3-year-olds talk about the future (e.g., Atance & O'Neill, 2005;Nelson, 1989), and that by about 4 years of age, children can begin to make decisions that benefit a future, but not a present, self (e.g., Moore et al, 1998). What the current study adds is information concerning children's reasoning about everyday events that will affect their future states.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We know that 2-and 3-year-olds talk about the future (e.g., Atance & O'Neill, 2005;Nelson, 1989), and that by about 4 years of age, children can begin to make decisions that benefit a future, but not a present, self (e.g., Moore et al, 1998). What the current study adds is information concerning children's reasoning about everyday events that will affect their future states.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Children are told that they can have the smaller reward immediately, or that they can wait to obtain the larger reward. In general, the ability to delay gratification in this context, as well as similar ones, increases with age (Mischel et al, 1989;Moore, Barresi, & Thompson, 1998;Thompson, Barresi, & Moore, 1997). For instance, Moore and his colleagues found that 4-and 5-year-olds delay choosing one sticker immediately, in favor of two stickers later, significantly more often than 3-yearolds.…”
Section: Delay Of Gratificationmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…However, by observing a coherent pattern of self-evaluative responses that are linked to both empathy and prosocial behavior, we demonstrate that when viewed 'online' young children's experience of self-evaluative emotion appears mature. For this reason, the lag in predicting complex emotion (and associated delays in sharing/altruistic behavior) might be explained not by deficits in self-evaluation, but by choices being compromised by young children's established difficulties in inhibition and forward thinking (Moore, Barresi & Thompson, 1998). Alternatively, the link between self-evaluation and prosociality may initially function on an implicit level, before later being made explicit in children's moral reasoning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contrary to the positive results for selfcontrol, early work often failed to find the predicted association between prosociality and perspective taking ability in children younger than 4 years (Denham, 1986; Iannoti, 1985;Underwood & Moore, 1982). Although several recent studies report a positive association between cognitive perspective taking and prosociality for 4-to 6-year-olds (Farrant, Devine, Maybery & Fletcher, 2012;Takagishi, Kameshima, Schug, Koizumi, & Yamagishi, 2010;Moore, Barresi, & Thompson, 1998;Wu & Su, 2014), null and even negative results continue to be observed (Imuta et al, 2016). The most recent meta-analysis concludes that cognitive perspective taking has a significant but small association with prosociality (Imuta et al, 2016).…”
Section: Research Highlightsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…As a general marker of unfavorable intrauterine conditions, low birth weight has been linked to various later-life outcomes ranging from cardiovascular disease (Barker, 1990) to lower income (Black et al, 2007) and reduced cognitive abilities (Hack et al, 2005), which in turn reduce the taste for cooperation (Moore et al, 1998;Zhang et al, 2015).…”
Section: Exogenous Shocks and Cooperationmentioning
confidence: 99%