“…Bilingual executive advantages have been observed in children (e.g., Bialystok & Barac, 2012; Kalashnikova & Mattock, 2014; Nicolay & Poncelet, 2013; Nicolay & Poncelet, 2015), adults (e.g., Bialystok, Poarch, Luo, & Craik 2014; Costa, Hernandez, Costa-Faidella, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2009; Costa, Hernandez, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2008; Fernandez, Acosta, Douglass, Doshi, & Tartar, 2014; Fernadez, Tartar, Padron, & Acosta, 2013; Ibrahim, Shoshani, Prior, Prior, & Share, 2013; Marzecová, Asanowicz, Kriva, & Wodniecka, 2012; Seçer, 2016) or even older adults (e.g., Bialystok et al, 2014). These advantages, demonstrated by several studies, have been noted in tasks assessing, for instance, the suppression of automatic response tendencies (response inhibition; Fernandez et al, 2013; Fernandez et al, 2014), the ability to ignore irrelevant conflicting information (interference inhibition; Costa et al, 2008; Marzecová et al, 2012), the ability to shift from a mental set to another (set-shifting) within the demands of a particular context or to switch attention from one aspect to another of a stimulus (cognitive flexibility; Ibrahim et al, 2013; Liu, Fan, Rossi, Yao, & Chen, 2015; Nicolay & Poncelet, 2013, 2015; Seçer, 2016), and the ability to monitor and to use (or to delete) working memory information (updating; e.g., Dong & Li, 2015). All these skills cover the range of executive sub-components (inhibition, cognitive shifting or cognitive flexibility, and updating) described by the model of Miyake (Miyake et al, 2000).…”