2018
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206724
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The complementary role of affect-based and cognitive heuristics to make decisions under conditions of ambivalence and complexity

Abstract: Little is known about the interplay between affective and cognitive processes of decision making within the bounded rationality perspective, in particular for the debate on adaptive decision making and strategy selection. This gap in the knowledge is particularly important as affect and deliberation may direct preferences in opposite directions. How do decision makers solve such dissonance? In this paper, we address this question by exploring the use of integral affect as a choice heuristic in comparison with … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Adding to prior research (Trujillo, 2018) which suggests that experienced emotion predicts judgment and decision-making more accurately than compensatory and noncompensatory cognitive models, the valence of predicted affective forecasts will better predict policy decision-making than will cue-based modeling regardless of the type of model (i.e., fast and frugal heuristics or full compensatory models).…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Adding to prior research (Trujillo, 2018) which suggests that experienced emotion predicts judgment and decision-making more accurately than compensatory and noncompensatory cognitive models, the valence of predicted affective forecasts will better predict policy decision-making than will cue-based modeling regardless of the type of model (i.e., fast and frugal heuristics or full compensatory models).…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 92%
“…We calculated separate cue utilization scores for each participant and display them along with their measurement properties in Table 3. Experiments which compare compensatory and noncompensatory models routinely use participant ratings to ascertain cue values, cue weights, and cue utilization scores (e.g., Dhami & Harries, 2001; Garcia-Retamero & Dhami, 2009; Trujillo, 2018; Wang, 2008).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The lack of cross-task convergence in affect heuristic use could have been influenced by methodological factors as well: Although all tasks were completed within a single test session by the same participants, indices derived from different affect heuristic tasks did not converge. Conceivably, reliance on the affect heuristic may be context-or stimulus-dependent rather than a stable inter-individual trait: As is the case for other heuristics, use of the affect heuristic increases under time pressure, when cognitive resources run low, or in the absence of explicit information (Fleming, Townsend, van Hilten, Spence, & Ferguson, 2012;Finucane et al, 2000;King & Slovic, 2014;Shiv & Fedorikhin, 1999;Trendel & Werle, 2016;Trujillo, 2018). For example, decision makers are more likely to choose emotionally favorable options over less favorable options when their cognitive load is high and the choice environment is complex (Shiv & Fedorikin, 1999;Trujillo, 2018).…”
Section: Convergence Across Task Typesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conceivably, reliance on the affect heuristic may be context-or stimulus-dependent rather than a stable inter-individual trait: As is the case for other heuristics, use of the affect heuristic increases under time pressure, when cognitive resources run low, or in the absence of explicit information (Fleming, Townsend, van Hilten, Spence, & Ferguson, 2012;Finucane et al, 2000;King & Slovic, 2014;Shiv & Fedorikhin, 1999;Trendel & Werle, 2016;Trujillo, 2018). For example, decision makers are more likely to choose emotionally favorable options over less favorable options when their cognitive load is high and the choice environment is complex (Shiv & Fedorikin, 1999;Trujillo, 2018). As a result, use of the affect heuristic may be sensitive to the specifics of the task design.…”
Section: Convergence Across Task Typesmentioning
confidence: 99%