2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The complexity of theory of mind

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

6
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This result seems to suggest that when player C watches player A acting unfairly, he tries to understand the intentions or goals behind the in-group member’s unfair behavior in order to justify it. Our results are in line with the relevant literature showing that these two brain regions are key components of the mentalizing network, involved in inferring goals, intentions, desires, as well as more enduring dispositions of others [2932], [27], [6365], [28]. In addition to this network, we observed increased activity of the caudate nucleus.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…This result seems to suggest that when player C watches player A acting unfairly, he tries to understand the intentions or goals behind the in-group member’s unfair behavior in order to justify it. Our results are in line with the relevant literature showing that these two brain regions are key components of the mentalizing network, involved in inferring goals, intentions, desires, as well as more enduring dispositions of others [2932], [27], [6365], [28]. In addition to this network, we observed increased activity of the caudate nucleus.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“… Frith (1992) was the first to propose that a deficit of ToM could explain the cognitive and behavioral abnormalities of schizophrenia. This hypothesis was widely confirmed (e.g., Corcoran et al, 1995 , 1997 ; Frith and Corcoran, 1996 ; Sarfati and Hardy-Baylé, 1999 ; Mazza et al, 2001 ; Brüne, 2005 ; Bosco et al, 2009 ). Specifically, Frith (1992) proposed to explain the communicative-pragmatic deficit of schizophrenia as the consequence of a primary ToM deficit.…”
Section: Empirical Studies Of the Relationship Between Pragmatics Andmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…As far as the relationship between ToM and pragmatics is concerned, several authors have stressed that the ability to understand someone’s mental states and their relation with behavior is an undisputed requirement of human communication ( Happé and Loth, 2002 ; Sperber and Wilson, 2002 ; Tirassa et al, 2006a , b ; Bosco et al, 2009 ; Cummings, 2015 ). For example, the ability to deceive in communication has been commonly explained based on the ability to understand and foresee the interlocutor’s mental states ( Peskin, 1996 ; Polak and Harris, 1999 ; Lee, 2000 ; Ma et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Empirical Studies Of the Relationship Between Pragmatics Andmentioning
confidence: 99%
“… Gallagher (2006) notes that ToM approaches to the explanation of how we come to understand others typically are abstract (third-person when they need to be second-person), mentalistic (starting with the supposition that there are things like minds, beliefs, desires that we have no access to in others, and sometimes even in ourselves), and biased toward theoretical reason (when practical, situated reason is a better way to go: see also Bosco et al, 2009b ). Overly intellectualizing what is involved in our basic encounters with others, they tend to forget emotion and our ability to read it not in the minds of others, but on their faces, as well as in their gestures and expressive movements.…”
Section: From Childhood To Adolescencementioning
confidence: 99%