2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The confluence of intrinsic and extrinsic constraints on 3- to 9-month-old infants’ catching behavior

Abstract: The present study sought to uncover what constraints shape the early development of the perceptual-motor skill of catching, and how intrinsic (i.e., movement possibilities) and extrinsic constraints (i.e., object speed imposes temporal precision) impinge on the perception of whether or not a moving object affords catching. Thirty-five infants (with different preferred catching tendencies) between 3 and 9 months of age were presented with balls approaching from the side with different speeds. Video recordings w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

2
14
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
2
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The fact that manual strategies when grasping a moving object change in a non-linear way during the first year of life is in line with other findings (van Hof et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The fact that manual strategies when grasping a moving object change in a non-linear way during the first year of life is in line with other findings (van Hof et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Contralateral grasping appears to be a common strategy which increases with the speed of the object (von Hofsten, 1980, 1983; von Hofsten & Lindhagen, 1979; van Hof, van der Kamp, Caljou, & Savelsbergh, 2005). In addition to switching hands after an ipsilateral reaching, using the contralateral hand to grasp a moving object can be the result of a contralateral reaching from the start.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…doi:10. 1016/j.infbeh.2006.07.006 in motion (Van Hof, Van der Kamp, Caljouw, & Savelsbergh, 2005) and different body orientations (Out, Van Soest, Savelsbergh, & Hopkins, 1998;Rochat, 1992;Savelsbergh & Van der Kamp, 1994).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intrinsic factors are considered to be: the child's age (Rocha, Silva, & Tudella, 2006a), experience in performing the task , the level of postural control (Fallang, Saugstad, & Hadders-Algra, 2000;Thelen & Spencer, 1998), and the presence of risk factors, such as prematurity (Plantinga, Perdok, & De Groot, 1997;Tudella & Toledo, 2008), and low birth weight (Chaudhari et al, 1999;Drummond & Colver, 2002;Martin et al, 2005). The extrinsic factors are considered to be body posture (Carvalho, Tudella, & Savelsbergh, 2007;Out, Van Soest, & Hopkins, 1998;Rochat, 1992;Savelsbergh & van der Kamp, 1994), the physical properties of the object (Rocha et al, 2006a;van Hof, van der Kamp, & Savelsbergh, 2006), the spatial orientation of the object (Lee, Liu, & Newell, 2006;van Hof, van der Kamp, Caljouw, & Savelsbergh, 2005), the additional weight on the child's upper limb (Out, Savelsbergh, Van Soest, & Hopkins, 1997;Toledo, Soares, & Tudella, 2012), as well as exposure to spontaneous (Carvalho, Gonçalves, & Tudella, 2008) and induced practice (training; Cunha, Woollacott, & Tudella, 2013;Heathcock et al, 2008;Lobo, Galloway, & Savelsbergh, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%