2015
DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12119
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The dark side of cognitive illusions: When an illusory belief interferes with the acquisition of evidence‐based knowledge

Abstract: Cognitive illusions are often associated with mental health and well‐being. However, they are not without risk. This research shows they can interfere with the acquisition of evidence‐based knowledge. During the first phase of the experiment, one group of participants was induced to develop a strong illusion that a placebo medicine was effective to treat a fictitious disease, whereas another group was induced to develop a weak illusion. Then, in Phase 2, both groups observed fictitious patients who always took… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
28
0
4

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
0
28
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…With respect to mood and locus of control, neither predicted causal ratings; in particular, no evidence of a depressive realism effect was observed (see also Allan, Siegel, & Hannah, 2007). The present data make an important additional contribution to the superstition literature by examining the operation of basic causal learning mechanisms that are often assumed to be related to superstitions (see Blanco et al, 2015;Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015). While numerous studies have examined whether particular groups of people are more or less likely to endorse superstitious beliefs (see Irwin, 1993 for a review), or have investigated general cognitive factors or deficits thought to be related to superstitious thinking (for reviews see French, 1992;Wiseman & Watt, 2006), there has been relatively little work examining the basic cognitive mechanisms by which people might arrive at superstitious or paranormal beliefs, and whether these cognitive mechanisms differ across individuals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 62%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With respect to mood and locus of control, neither predicted causal ratings; in particular, no evidence of a depressive realism effect was observed (see also Allan, Siegel, & Hannah, 2007). The present data make an important additional contribution to the superstition literature by examining the operation of basic causal learning mechanisms that are often assumed to be related to superstitions (see Blanco et al, 2015;Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015). While numerous studies have examined whether particular groups of people are more or less likely to endorse superstitious beliefs (see Irwin, 1993 for a review), or have investigated general cognitive factors or deficits thought to be related to superstitious thinking (for reviews see French, 1992;Wiseman & Watt, 2006), there has been relatively little work examining the basic cognitive mechanisms by which people might arrive at superstitious or paranormal beliefs, and whether these cognitive mechanisms differ across individuals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 62%
“…Participants reporting high levels of superstitious beliefs tended to rate the button press as having caused the light to illuminatea causal illusionwhereas those with low superstition were less susceptible to this illusion and tended to (correctly) see the events as unrelated. The present data make an important additional contribution to the superstition literature by examining the operation of basic causal learning mechanisms that are often assumed to be related to superstitions (see Blanco et al, 2015;Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015). The relationship held both for the magnitude of people's causal judgements (i.e., the degree of causality people saw the button press as possessing) and also the propensity to infer causation (i.e., whether people saw a causal relationship between the two events, irrespective of its magnitude).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Specifically, participants were told that over a set of discrete trials they could test whether their actions caused a given outcome. In a zero-contingency task, programmed so that the outcome happened equally Individual Differences 36 often in response to and in the absence of the participants' actions, non-depressed participants tended to overestimate the contingency between action and outcome, while individuals with mild symptoms of depression accurately judged the zero contingency (see also, Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015;Yarritu, Matute, & Vadillo, 2014).…”
Section: Individual Differences 35mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, participants were told that over a set of discrete trials they could test whether their actions caused a given outcome. In a zero-contingency task, programmed so that the outcome happened equally Individual Differences 36 often in response to and in the absence of the participants' actions, non-depressed participants tended to overestimate the contingency between action and outcome, while individuals with mild symptoms of depression accurately judged the zero contingency (see also, Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015;Yarritu, Matute, & Vadillo, 2014).A normative measure of the contingency between action and outcome (ΔP), describes the strength and direction of a contingent relationship in terms of the difference between the probability of the outcome occurring following an action, P(o|e) and the probability of the outcome occurring in the absence of the action (i.e., effect), P(o|~e), as shown in Figure 3 (Allan, 1980). Judgements of control are broadly sensitive to manipulations of the statistical contingency between an action and an outcome (e.g., Baker, Murphy, Vallee-Tourangeau, 1996;Shanks, 1987;Wasserman, Elek, Chatlosh & Baker, 1993).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, as diverse technology becomes increasingly applied to sport performance, coaches will need to understand and focus more on the crucial mechanisms that are accurate and most informative towards the task at hand. Once again, this will not be an easy challenge since it is difficult for individuals to depart from a strongly held view, even when it is to be replaced by one with a strong evidence base (Yarritu, Matute, & Luque, 2015).…”
Section: Too Many Cooks?mentioning
confidence: 99%