2010
DOI: 10.1080/17470910903523327
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development and neural basis of pointing comprehension

Abstract: The neurological correlates of pointing comprehension in adults and 8-month-old infants are explored. Both age groups demonstrate differential activation to congruent and incongruent pointing gestures over posterior temporal areas. The functional similarity of the adult N200 and the infant P400 component suggests that they might have a common source.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

3
59
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 86 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
3
59
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This difference was significant despite the fact that the infants did not demonstrate any behavioral responses to the give‐me gesture. Additional studies have also found that the P400 component has expressed sensitivity to typical and referential cues, such as congruent pointing (Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, ), and congruent eye‐gaze (Senju, Johnson, & Csibra, ), compared to incongruent control stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This difference was significant despite the fact that the infants did not demonstrate any behavioral responses to the give‐me gesture. Additional studies have also found that the P400 component has expressed sensitivity to typical and referential cues, such as congruent pointing (Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, ), and congruent eye‐gaze (Senju, Johnson, & Csibra, ), compared to incongruent control stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Therefore, it is unclear whether 1) infants understand how the listener should respond to the speaker’s speech (the outcome of the event), and 2) whether infants use that knowledge to anticipate the listener’s response to speech in the same way they do for gestures. Given that infants within their first year seem to understand that non-verbal gestures and speech function similarly in communicative scenes (e.g., Csibra & Gergely, 2009, 2011; Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, 2010; Martin et al, 2012; Vouloumanos et al, 2014), 12-month-olds may interpret speech like gesture in these events: by making predictions about the outcome during the communicative event.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We showed participants a video of pre-recorded actions in a third-party scenario (Augusti et al, 2010; Fawcett & Gredebäck, 2013; Gredebäck et al, 2010; Thorgrimsson et al, 2015) during which an actor (the Communicator), alone, repeatedly grasped one object (target) rather than another object (non-target), suggesting that the Communicator may prefer the target object (e.g., Cannon & Woodward, 2012; Woodward, 1998). In the test scene, the Communicator and a second actor (the Listener) were both present.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It would reveal whether it is the product of learning processes or whether children exhibit such a preference from the moment they engage in point following (that is, around or before their 1st birthday; cf. Behne, Liszkowski, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2012;Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, 2010). A shift in the preferential reliance from gaze to pointing cues could indicate a shift from often relatively automatic and reflex-like processes (cf.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%